Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts

Research articles

nature.com research paper

Prognostic value of platelet levels in patients with aneurysmal Subarachnoid Hemorrhage

  • Igor Fischer
  • Ronahi Lala
  • Sajjad Muhammad

nature.com research paper

The N400 effect captures nuances in implicit political preferences

  • Emmanuel Mahieux
  • Nicole Y. Y. Wicha

nature.com research paper

Influence of environmental, geographic, socio-demographic, and epidemiological factors on presence of malaria at the community level in two continents

  • Oswaldo C. Villena
  • Leah R. Johnson

nature.com research paper

Interaction between ambient CO and temperature or relative humidity on the risk of stroke hospitalization

  • Peifeng Liang

nature.com research paper

Retrospective analysis of interpretable machine learning in predicting ICU thrombocytopenia in geriatric ICU patients

  • Yingting Xu
  • Weimin Zhang
  • Xuandong Jiang

nature.com research paper

Detection and characterization of Deformed Wing Virus (DWV) in apiaries with stationary and migratory management in the province of Entre Ríos, Argentina

  • F. N. Gonzalez
  • F. Raticelli
  • M. J. Dus Santos

nature.com research paper

Development and external validation of a predictive model for type 2 diabetic retinopathy

  • Yongsheng Li

nature.com research paper

The variation landscape of CYP2D6 in a multi-ethnic Asian population

  • Yusuf Maulana
  • Rodrigo Toro Jimenez
  • Mar Gonzalez-Porta

nature.com research paper

Machine learning analysis of PM1 impact on visibility with comprehensive sensitivity evaluation of concentration, composition, and meteorological factors

  • Grzegorz Majewski
  • Bartosz Szeląg
  • Barbara Klik

nature.com research paper

Exploring the collision, acoustic and thermal energy dissipation distribution of discrete mass

  • Zhang Shengwu
  • Wang Chiming
  • Zhu Shunzhi

nature.com research paper

Exploring the therapeutic potential of Aloin: unraveling neuroprotective and anticancer mechanisms, and strategies for enhanced stability and delivery

  • Stefania Zimbone
  • Valeria Romanucci
  • Danilo Milardi

nature.com research paper

Risk of chronic kidney disease in patients with a hyperglycemic crisis as the initial presentation of type 2 diabetes

  • Chun-Ta Huang
  • Chih-Hsin Muo
  • Pei-Chun Chen

nature.com research paper

Cost-utility and budget impact analysis of neoadjuvant dual HER2 targeted therapy for HER2-positive breast cancer in Sri Lanka

  • Agampodi Danushi Mendis Gunasekara
  • Sitaporn Youngkong
  • Usa Chaikledkaew

nature.com research paper

Nasal microbiome and the effect of nasal decolonization with a novel povidone-iodine antiseptic solution: a prospective and randomized clinical trial

  • Diana Fernández-Rodríguez
  • Jeongeun Cho
  • Javad Parvizi

nature.com research paper

Plant-associated halotolerant bacteria improving growth of Vicia faba L. Mariout-2 under salinity conditions

  • Yasser El-Amier
  • Ashraf Elsayed

nature.com research paper

Characterization of fully-evaporated perovskite solar cells and photodetectors under high-intensity pulsed proton irradiation

  • Hryhorii P. Parkhomenko
  • Andriy I. Mostovyi
  • Viktor V. Brus

nature.com research paper

Protective effects of patchouli alcohol against DSS-induced ulcerative colitis

  • Huifang Han
  • Hongyang Zhang

nature.com research paper

A study of the effects of early diagenesis on the geotechnical properties of carbonate sediments (North West Shelf, Australia)

  • Ulysse Lebrec
  • Shambhu Sharma
  • Christophe Gaudin

nature.com research paper

GSHFA-HCP: a novel intelligent high-performance clustering protocol for agricultural IoT in fragrant pear production monitoring

nature.com research paper

Neonatal hyperoxia exposure leads to developmental programming of cardiovascular and renal disease in adult rats

  • Marissa J. DeFreitas
  • Elaine L. Shelton
  • Merline Benny

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

nature.com research paper

nature.com research paper

  • Chinese (Traditional)
  • Nature Support
  • Solution home
  • Author and Peer Reviewer Support
  • After Acceptance and Production

Find submission status of your article / manuscript

As the corresponding or submitting author , you can track the progress of your article online via Your Submission (login required). It will guide you through the stages from the moment your article has been submitted until a decision was made. 

Every step is described and will let you know whether any action is required, which you can then perform as appropriate by clicking on the respective manuscript, leading you to the submission system. 

You can also access it via the starting page of your Springer Nature Profile where you will need to login and navigate to "Your Research". Here, you will see the segment "Track the progress of your submitted articles", where you will see the status of your latest submission.

Underneath, you will see "Track all submissions" where you will find a list with all of your submissions:

A screenshot showing where to click "Track all submissions".

Please note that the email address you are registered with under your Springer Nature Profile needs to match the email you used during submission. 

If you still need to get a (matching) account, please create one under register a new account, to track your submissions and check your impact . 

To learn what happens after submission, please check Editorial process after submission . 

If you have any further questions regarding your article submission and its status or your submission is not visible, please check the contact mentioned in the submission system or Who to contact with publishing enquiries .

Upon publication, the corresponding author will receive an email including a link to their downloadable article PDF. 

All authors can also use our SharedIt feature to view and share their published article, outlined in Content sharing .

Related Articles

  • Next steps for publishing your article: What to expect after acceptance
  • Timescale to publish an article for a Springer Nature journal
  • Author Copyrights information
  • Book manuscript tracking
  • Author rights information
  • Page numbers in a Continuous Article Publishing (CAP) Journal
  • Next steps for publishing your article: How to confirm your affiliation
  • Next steps for publishing your article: How to choose a publishing model (Open access or subscription)

Article views count

Scientists discover a cause of lupus and a possible way to reverse it

A gloved hand holds a vial of blood

  • Feinberg School of Medicine

The autoimmune disease systemic lupus erythematosus — known as lupus — affects more than 1.5 million people in the U.S. It can result in life-threatening damage to multiple organs including the kidneys, brain and heart. The causes of this disease have long been unclear. Existing treatments often fail to control the disease, the study authors said, and have unintended side effects of reducing the immune system’s ability to fight infections.

But now, Northwestern Medicine and Brigham and Women’s Hospital scientists have discovered a molecular defect that promotes the pathologic immune response in lupus and show that reversing this defect may potentially reverse the disease.

“Up until this point, all therapy for lupus is a blunt instrument. It’s broad immunosuppression,” said co-corresponding author Dr. Jaehyuk Choi, associate professor of dermatology at Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine and a Northwestern Medicine dermatologist. “By identifying a cause for this disease, we have found a potential cure that will not have the side effects of current therapies.”

“We’ve identified a fundamental imbalance in the immune responses that patients with lupus make, and we’ve defined specific mediators that can correct this imbalance to dampen the pathologic autoimmune response,” said co-corresponding author Dr. Deepak Rao, an assistant professor of medicine at Harvard Medical School and a rheumatologist at Brigham and Women’s Hospital and co-director of its Center for Cellular Profiling.

In a study published in Nature on July 10, the scientists report a new pathway that drives disease in lupus. There are disease-associated changes in multiple molecules in the blood of patients with lupus. Ultimately, these changes lead to insufficient activation of a pathway controlled by the aryl hydrocarbon receptor (AHR), which regulates cells’ response to environmental pollutants, bacteria or metabolites, a substance created when the body breaks down food, drugs, chemicals or its own tissue. Insufficient activation of AHR results in too many immune cells that promote the production of disease-causing autoantibodies.

To show this discovery can be leveraged for treatments, the investigators returned the AHR-activating molecules to blood samples from lupus patients. This seemed to reprogram these lupus-causing cells into a type of cell that may promote wound healing from the damage caused by this autoimmune disease.

“We found that if we either activate the AHR pathway with small molecule activators or limit the pathologically excessive interferon in the blood, we can reduce the number of these disease-causing cells,” said Choi, also the Jack W. Graffin Professor at Feinberg. “If these effects are durable, this may be a potential cure.”

Choi, Rao and colleagues next want to expand their efforts into developing novel treatments for lupus patients. They are now working to find ways to deliver these molecules safely and effectively to people.

Other Northwestern authors are first author Calvin Law, Arundhati Pillai, Brandon Hancock and Dr. Judd Hultquist.

Editor’s Picks

deering library

Deering Library undergoing major renovations

Northwestern celebrates the groundbreaking of new ryan field, forget imitation crab — researchers test snackable snails, related stories.

phosphorus

Minerals play newly discovered role in Earth’s phosphorus cycle

Creating artificial ‘muscles’ for safer, softer robots, firearm-violence public health crisis ‘a wake-up call’.

Research Communities by Springer Nature

Behind the paper.

The real stories behind the latest research papers, from conception to publication, the highs and the lows

Global Research on Osteoarthritis During 1994–2023: A Scientometric Assessment of Publications and Citations

Go to the profile of Dr. (Prof.) Raju Vaishya

Removing carbon dioxide from the air using a humidity-driven membrane

Go to the profile of Greg A. Mutch

Protein Structure Mapping: Unveiling the Mechanism of Diazirine Photo-reactions

Go to the profile of Yida Jiang

Synergistic induction of mitotic pyroptosis and tumor remission by inhibiting proteasome and WEE family kinases

Go to the profile of Shi-Mei Zhuang

SANTO: a coarse-to-fine alignment and stitching method for spatial omics

Go to the profile of Haoyang Li

To Die or To Fuse? Fine-tuned apoptotic signals decide over cellular fate in osteoclastogenesis

Go to the profile of Brenda Krishnacoumar

Uncovering Genetic Clues in ADHD Through DNA Sequencing

Go to the profile of Thomas Fernandez, MD

Nucleation phenomena and extreme vulnerability of spatial k-core systems

Go to the profile of Leyang Xue

Why people predict backward or forward

Go to the profile of Paul Sharp

More than a than a battle of males: the association between alpha male takeovers, social networks, and gut microbiota composition

Go to the profile of Eva Wikberg

Methane oxidation to formaldehyde via photon–phonon cascade catalysis

Go to the profile of Youxun Xu

Mechanical regulation of cell-nanoparticle interactions

Go to the profile of Oleg Lunov, PhD

Characterizing amphioxus development at the single-cell level: understanding chordate nervous system development

Go to the profile of Yichen (Serena) Dai

The journey to SPA for volumetric structured illumination microscopy

Go to the profile of Kenta Temma

Transient Shuttle for a Widespread Neural Probe with Minimal Perturbation

Go to the profile of EUNA KIM

Memristive circuits based on multilayer hexagonal boron nitride for radiofrequency and millimetre wave applications

Go to the profile of Mario Lanza

Bionic E-skin with Precise Multi-Directional Droplet Sliding Sensing for Enhanced Robotic Perception

Go to the profile of Yunlong Xu

Sharing is Innovating: Transparent Data Collection and Processing with DevStart

Go to the profile of Francesco Poli

One step closer to preventing invasive E. coli disease, including sepsis and bacteremia.

Go to the profile of Jan Poolman

Seed dispersal by Martu peoples promotes the distribution of native plants in arid Australia

Go to the profile of Rebecca Bliege Bird

  • « First
  • ‹ Prev
  • Next ›
  • Last »

We use cookies to ensure the functionality of our website, to personalize content and advertising, to provide social media features, and to analyze our traffic. If you allow us to do so, we also inform our social media, advertising and analysis partners about your use of our website. You can decide for yourself which categories you want to deny or allow. Please note that based on your settings not all functionalities of the site are available.

Further information can be found in our privacy policy .

Cookie Control

Customise your preferences for any tracking technology

The following allows you to customize your consent preferences for any tracking technology used to help us achieve the features and activities described below. To learn more about how these trackers help us and how they work, refer to the cookie policy. You may review and change your preferences at any time.

These trackers are used for activities that are strictly necessary to operate or deliver the service you requested from us and, therefore, do not require you to consent.

These trackers help us to deliver personalized marketing content and to operate, serve and track ads.

These trackers help us to deliver personalized marketing content to you based on your behaviour and to operate, serve and track social advertising.

These trackers help us to measure traffic and analyze your behaviour with the goal of improving our service.

These trackers help us to provide a personalized user experience by improving the quality of your preference management options, and by enabling the interaction with external networks and platforms.

  • Share full article

Advertisement

Supported by

Concerned About Metals in Tampons? Here’s What to Know.

A recent study detected a number of metals in many types of tampons. Experts weigh in on the findings and what they could mean for your health.

Six tampons sit on a blue background, photographed from overhead.

By Dana G. Smith and Katie Mogg

A new study about tampons has sparked concern among consumers. The research, published in the journal Environment International, found evidence of 16 different metals in a wide range of tampons sold in the United States and Europe.

In the days since news of the study came out, women have taken to social media to express their alarm about the findings. “Soo nice of them to let us know they found LEAD & ARSENIC in our tampons 20 years after I started my period,” one TikTok user posted .

But while these findings can sound scary, several experts, including one of the scientists who led the research, said they are not a reason to panic, nor to immediately stop using tampons.

Levels of the toxic chemicals detected, including arsenic and lead, were very low, and two prior studies by the same group of researchers did not find significantly higher blood levels of the metals in women who use tampons compared with those who don’t.

“There’s small amounts of these metals found, really, everywhere,” said Dr. Bethany Samuelson Bannow, an associate professor of medicine specializing in hematology at Oregon Health and Science University, who was not involved in the study. “I would actually be less concerned about having these metals in tampons than I would about having them in food or water.”

Here’s what to know about the research and what it might mean for your health.

What the study found

The researchers analyzed 30 tampons from 14 different brands for arsenic, barium, calcium, cadmium, cobalt, chromium, copper, iron, manganese, mercury, nickel, lead, selenium, strontium, vanadium and zinc.

We are having trouble retrieving the article content.

Please enable JavaScript in your browser settings.

Thank you for your patience while we verify access. If you are in Reader mode please exit and  log into  your Times account, or  subscribe  for all of The Times.

Thank you for your patience while we verify access.

Already a subscriber?  Log in .

Want all of The Times?  Subscribe .

springernature press site

Welcome to the springer nature press site.

The Springer Nature journals press office promotes research and other content published in the Nature Portfolio, BMC, Palgrave Macmillan and Springer titles in the Springer Nature portfolio. Our press site provides journalists with embargoed access to press releases, full-text PDFs of upcoming articles, and accompanying images, videos, commentaries and more detailed releases from authors and their institutions.

If you already have a Springer Nature press site account, please log in here .

If you are a working journalist and would like to register for access to our press site and to receive emailed press releases about our journals, please click the “apply to register” link below. As part of the registration process, you will be asked to provide documentation to support your status as a journalist.

Apply to register

  • Press releases
  • Climate change
  • Free journals access
  • Registered Media FAQs
  • For authors

A Nature Research Service

Scientific Writing and Publishing

For students and researchers in the natural sciences who are new to scientific writing or publishing or who wish to refresh their skills

Subscriptions available to institutions, departments and labs

Key features

3 on-demand courses  with certificates

36 Editors  Scientific writing and publishing experts, including Nature Portfolio Editors

21   hours  of learning

10-minute lessons

English language captions and transcripts

Skills gained

Develop writing skills and confidence writing for journals

Understand editorial processes and what editors look for

Learn best practices for submitting a paper and peer review

What you will learn in the three courses

Writing a research paper: 2nd edition.

Learn the detailed processes of writing a research paper

Publishing a Research Paper

Learn about the publication process and the things you need to consider

Writing and Publishing a Review Paper

Prepare yourself to write and publish a great review paper

Delivered by Nature Portfolio journal Editors

The courses are delivered by 30  Editors from over 20 Nature Portfolio journals  - giving researchers an unparalleled insight into the publishing and manuscript selection process.

“It was excellent to hear from the editors, personally, about what they are looking for.” 

Principal Investigator, Canada

Dip in and out design

Researchers can work at their own pace through the three courses - whether that is working through the courses in order or dipping in and out of modules. There are three courses, and content is easy to find. Each course has its own certificate of completion. The lessons are bite-sized (around 10 mins) and easy to fit around a busy research schedule.

“The shortness of the videos allows me to fit them in between work and research. This format also meant that I could take in the information bit by bit...” 

Post-doctoral student, United States

Individual researcher?

Recommend us.

Recommend the course to a lab, department, or institution.

FREE SAMPLE

Register for free to access a sample of the online course. 

Subscriptions

Laboratory subscription.

  • Up to 15 researchers
  • 12 months’ access
  • Usage reports showing researcher uptake
  • Discounts for multi-year

Price: Request a quote

Departmental subscription

  • Up to 50 researchers
  • Promotional resources
  • Discounts for multi-year  

Institutional subscription

  • Institution-wide access
  • Launch presentation

Subscribers worldwide

Institutions are subscribing to our 'Scientific Writing and Publishing' courses to support their researchers in getting published. 

Discover related courses

Focus on peer review online course.

Explore the role of a peer reviewer and the foundations of a good peer review

Live, editor-led workshops in scientific writing and publishing

One- and two-day workshops, available to institutions worldwide

Image Credits

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Life Sci Soc Policy
  • v.9; 2013 Dec

Logo of lssp

Perceptions of nature, nurture and behaviour

Mairi levitt.

Department of Politics, Philosophy and Religious Studies, Lancaster University, County South, Lancaster, LA1 4YL UK

Trying to separate out nature and nurture as explanations for behaviour, as in classic genetic studies of twins and families, is now said to be both impossible and unproductive. In practice the nature-nurture model persists as a way of framing discussion on the causes of behaviour in genetic research papers, as well as in the media and lay debate. Social and environmental theories of crime have been dominant in criminology and in public policy while biological theories have been seen as outdated and discredited. Recently, research into genetic variations associated with aggressive and antisocial behaviour has received more attention in the media. This paper explores ideas on the role of nature and nurture in violent and antisocial behaviour through interviews and open-ended questionnaires among lay publics. There was general agreement that everybody’s behaviour is influenced to varying degrees by both genetic and environmental factors but deterministic accounts of causation, except in exceptional circumstances, were rejected. Only an emphasis on nature was seen as dangerous in its consequences, for society and for individuals themselves. Whereas academic researchers approach the debate from their disciplinary perspectives which may or may not engage with practical and policy issues, the key issue for the public was what sort of explanations of behaviour will lead to the best outcomes for all concerned.

Trying to separate out nature and nurture as explanations for behaviour, as in classic genetic studies of twins and families, is now said to be both impossible and unproductive. The nature-nurture debate is declared to be officially redundant by social scientists and scientists, ‘outdated, naive and unhelpful’ (Craddock, 2011 , p.637), ‘a false dichotomy’ (Traynor 2010 , p.196). Geneticists argue that nature and nurture interact to affect behaviour through complex and not yet fully understood ways, but, in practice, the debate continues 1 . Research papers by psychologists and geneticists still use the terms nature and nurture, or genes and environment, to consider their relative influences on, for example, temperament and personality, childhood obesity and toddler sleep patterns (McCrae et al., 2000 ; Anderson et al., 2007 ; Brescianini, 2011 ). These papers separate out and quantify the relative influences of nature/genes and nurture/environment. These papers might be taken to indicate how individuals acquire their personality traits or toddlers acquire their sleep patterns; part is innate or there at birth and part is acquired after birth due to environmental influences. The findings actually refer to technical heritability which is, ‘the proportion of phenotypic variation attributable to genetic differences between individuals’ (Keller, 2010 , p.57). In practice, as Keller illustrates, there is ‘slippage’ between heritability, meaning a trait being biologically transmissible, and technical heritability. This is not simply a mistake made by the media or ‘media hype’ but is, she argues, ‘almost impossible to avoid’ (ibid, p.71).

While researchers are aware of the complexity of gene-environment interaction, the ‘nature and nurture’ model persists as a simple way of framing discussion on the causes of behaviours. It is also a site of struggle between (and within) academic disciplines and, through influence on policy, has consequences for those whose behaviours are investigated. There is general agreement between social scientists and geneticists about the past abuses of genetics but disagreement over whether it will be possible for the new behavioural genetics to avoid discrimination and eugenic practices, and about the likely benefits that society will gain from this research (Parens et al. 2006 , xxi). In a special issue of the American Journal of Sociology ‘Exploring genetics and social structure’, Bearman considers the reasons why sociologists are concerned about genetic effects on behaviour; first they see it as legitimating existing societal arrangements, which assumes that ‘genetic’ is unchangeable. Second, if sociologists draw on genetic research it contaminates the sociological enterprise and, third, whatever claims are made to the contrary, it is a eugenicist project (Bearman, 2008 , vi). As we will see all these concerns were expressed by the publics in this study. Policy makers and publics are interested in explaining problem behaviour in order to change/control it, not in respecting disciplinary boundaries, and will expect the role of genetics to be considered alongside social factors. 2

Social and environmental theories of criminal behaviour have been dominant in criminology, and in public policy (Walsh, 2009 , p.7). Genetic disorders and mental illness have provided explanations for a small minority of offenders with specific conditions. A 2007 survey of American criminologists found that ‘criminologists of all ideological persuasions view alleged biosocial causes of crime (hormonal, genetic, and evolutionary factors and possibly low intelligence) as relatively unimportant’ compared with environmental causes (Cooper et al., 2010 ). Sociology textbooks have typically discussed biological theories of criminality only as discredited (Haralambos and Holborn, 2004 , Giddens, 2009 ). Biosocial theories are seen as attractive to ‘agents of social control’ and to be more likely to lead to abusive treatment of offenders. However, with increasing research and public interest in genetics more attention has been paid to biological aspects of crime and to genetic variations within the normal range. Research has focussed on violent and antisocial behaviours which are criminal or may be seen as a precursor to criminal behaviour, for example, antisocial behaviour in young people. Media reports have headlined ‘warrior genes’, ‘the aggressive gene’ and the ‘get out of jail free gene’, all referring to levels of monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) (Lea and Chambers, 2007 ; Levitt and Pieri, 2009 ) 3 . Think tanks and ethics groups have considered the ethics and practicalities of genetic testing for behavioural traits (Campbell and Ross, 2004 ; Dixon, 2005 Nuffield Council on Bioethics, 2002 ).

An attraction of research into genes and behaviour is the hope that identifying a genetic factor that is correlated with an increased incidence of, say, violent and antisocial behaviour, will point to a way of reducing such behaviour. Fotaki discusses the attraction of biological explanations of inequalities in health based on the assumption that genetic interventions ‘would succeed in addressing the causes of ill health that public health policies cannot.’ (Fotaki, 2011 , p.641). The danger is that biological explanations ‘are once more employed for political purposes to explain away the social roots of health inequalities.’ (ibid). Social scientists, and criminologists, have presented biological/genetic explanations of behaviour as dangerous in terms of their potential effect on the individuals or groups identified as genetically at risk. There are obvious dangers of discrimination against, and the stigmatisation of, already vulnerable groups who would be the first to be tested i.e. ‘problem’ families or minority ethnic groups. Discrimination could affect education, employment and family life. The effect of an individual being told s/he has a risk based on a genetic test has been much discussed in relation to health risks (Claassen et al., 2010 . While such information could be motivating, because it is personalised, it can also induce a fatalistic attitude that discourages the person from taking preventative measures. Claasen et al. conclude that it is important to identify those vulnerable to the fatalistic impact and to tailor health risk information (ibid p.194). Identifying risk for behaviour, rather than for disease, is likely to be more problematic because of the difficulty of finding preventative measures that are within the individuals’ own control.

..using DNA to assess risk, make a diagnosis or tailor treatments, may weaken beliefs in the efficacy of preventive behaviour and reinforce biological ways of reducing risk, resulting in a preference for medication as opposed to behavioural means to control or reduce risk (ibid, xiv).

Claasen et al.’s comment on genetic tests for health conditions could apply equally to parents given a behavioural risk for their young child from a genetic test, perhaps before any problem behaviour was evident. The test result could weaken parents’ belief that they could take action to prevent/reduce the risk of the behaviour developing in their child and pharmaceutical solutions, as posited by Caspi et al. might not be available (Caspi et al., 2002 , xvii). However, it is not necessarily the case that evidence of genetic or biological influence on behaviour leads to more punitive treatment. DeLisi et al. give the example of the use of findings from adolescent brain science in the case of Roper v. Simmons in the US which abolished the death penalty for adolescents. On the basis of the research it was stated that young people under the age of 18 ‘are more vulnerable or susceptible to negative influences and outside pressures including peer pressure’ (DeLisa et al., 2010 , p.25) When evidence on genetic traits associated with criminal behaviour has been allowed by courts, mainly in the US, it has so far more often been accepted as a mitigating rather than an aggravating factor in the offenders’ behaviour (Denno, 2009 , Farahany and Coleman, 2006 ).

Environmental explanations of behaviour can, of course, also be presented as deterministic, claiming a closed future for those experiencing poverty and disadvantage. However, it is biological explanations that have caused more concern not only because of the history of eugenics but also because they may be seen as more fundamental, being there from birth, and as harder to change. The public in surveys are reported to see the greatest role for genetic factors in physical features, a lesser role in health conditions and a smaller role still in human behaviour (Condit, 2010 , p.619).

Public perceptions

The model of nature/genes and nurture/environment is still used in behavioural genetics, as well as in popular culture, and has implications for public policy, including the treatment of offenders who claim that a genetic trait has influenced their criminal behaviour. The aim of this research was to explore ideas on the causes of behaviour, particularly violent and antisocial behaviour and examine how respondents use the nature/nurture model. This qualitative research looks at the ways in which lay publics in different age groups conceptualise the factors and influences that made them who they are and their explanations for the behaviour of other people; especially violent behaviour. It was hypothesised that the increased research and media emphasis on the role of genetic factors in health and behaviour might result in an increasing interest in ‘nature’, biology and genes as explanations for behaviour particularly among the young, but, when explaining their own behaviour people might prefer to see themselves as agents with control over their lives. By exploring explanations of behaviour with respondents from different generations, age differences should be apparent.

The views of 78 respondents from 3 generations were gathered by individual interview and questionnaires, using the same open ended questions and responses to two real-life criminal court case studies where environmental or genetic factors had been used by the defence team. Respondents were drawn from a group of retired people participating in an informal ‘senior learners’ programme at Lancaster University, a group of their mainly younger relatives and, in order to recruit more third generation respondents, a group of first year students taking a criminology module. The senior learners group had a programme of talks and discussions and could attend undergraduate lectures. They had, by definition, shown an interest in current issues in a range of fields. There were no educational or age requirements for the group but all the volunteers were retired from paid work and were aged from around 65 years to over 80 years.. They had had similar careers to those popular with social science students; social work, probation, teaching and administrative positions. The senior learners were asked to pass on questionnaires to younger relatives to investigate age differences in attitudes. The first 13 senior learners who responded were interviewed but as only 15 questionnaires were received from their relatives ethical approval was obtained to distribute the same questionnaire to Lancaster University students taking the criminology first year module. Most students were enrolled on social science degrees, including psychology and sociology, and age 18 or 19. While the sample of senior learners and relatives had only a few more women than men, 78 per cent of the students were female reflecting the gender balance on the module as a whole. This makes it difficult to comment on any gender differences in responses. No claims to generalisability are made for this exploratory study. Responses were coded and entered on SPSS and also analysed thematically using Atlas-ti.

The introduction to the interviews and questionnaire was ‘I am interested in your views and ideas on what makes us the people we are; what makes people behave the way they do? What is the influence of nature and nurture?’ The terms, nature and nurture were not used again until the final question. Although the terms were not defined all respondents readily used them with consistent meanings. They identified ‘nature’ with biology, ‘what you are born with’ and genes or DNA and nurture with all aspects of the environment including parenting, socio-economic conditions, the food you eat, culture and other people. Their understanding of environment was therefore similar to that used by genetic researchers; environment as everything that is external to the individual, although they tended to refer more to the social than the biological environment.

A general warm-up question asked whether, in their own family, there was anything they thought of as a ‘family trait’. Then respondents were asked; ‘Imagine a baby swapped at birth and brought up in a completely different family– which influences do you think would be most important – the influence of the birth parents or the influences of the new family- and why?’ 4 The rest of the interview schedule, and the subsequent questionnaire, consisted of open-ended questions.

Respondents were asked how they would explain different kinds of behaviour if they came across a child who is kind and considerate; a young person who displays antisocial and aggressive behaviour adult and an adult with criminal convictions for violence. This was to tap into any differences in general explanations of good and bad behaviour in young people and adults. A quotation about the child killers in the Bulger case being ‘unreformable’ was used to ascertain whether they saw some types of violent behavior, and the actors concerned, as immutable. In order to see how respondents conceptualized the influences of nature/biology/genes and environment/people/experiences in their own lives, respondents were asked to write down ‘what or who made you what you are today’ and any explanation of their responses. Comments were gathered on the introduction of an environmental factor (childhood neglect) by the defence in a violent attack by two young boys in England, and on a genetic factor (MAOA levels) introduced by the defence in an criminal court in Italy. Respondents were asked how they thought such evidence should be dealt with; whether it should affect the degree of blame and whether it should affect criminal responsibility. The final question asked if it mattered ‘for individuals or society’ whether nature or nurture was seen as most important in explaining problem behaviour. Those interviewed were asked if they had any further comments and there was a space for any additional comments on the questionnaire.

This paper focuses on the ways in which respondents employed nature/genes and nurture/environment in their responses as a whole and what other concepts they drew on when explaining behaviour.

Respondents’ explanations of what makes people behave the way they do are discussed through three themes.

  • Nurture is more influential than nature
  • Nature and nurture interact
  • Emphasising nature (but never nurture) can be dangerous

Theme 1: Nurture is more influential than nature

Whether asked about influences on a baby adopted at birth, on their own lives, on an aggressive child or a violent young person, almost all respondents emphasised nurture. Parents and family were seen as the most important influences for babies and young children, moving to peer group and other relationships and experiences for a young person. The explanation for the violent behaviour of an adult had more to do with the individual and the importance of nurture/environment in explaining behaviour weakened. The quotations below explaining behaviour in a child adopted at birth, a young person and an adult illustrate the widening of influences from infancy through childhood and the onus on adults to take responsibility for themselves.

[a child] The environment in which a child grows up in, particularly the influence and role of the parents shapes how a child will grow up and what sort of adult they will be (77 Student). [a young person] I believe that upbringing shapes a person’s personality. Provisions of education, lifestyle opportunities and friendship groups all determine ….outlook. You can see evidence in young people at the school I teach at (20 Relative). Once adult they have to take responsibility for themselves and address whatever has been in their background. An adult can’t turn round and say it’s not my fault (5 Senior Learner).

Participants also saw themselves as shaped by the people surrounding them, starting with their parents, or those who brought them up. Several mentioned the illness and/or death of a parent during their childhood and older respondents talked about separation due to the second world war. Students were especially likely to mention the influence of morals instilled in them by their parents, the core values and discipline that they were taught at home. Educational experiences were important to all. For the senior learners the school leaving age had been age 15, so whether or not they stayed on at school and took public examinations was crucial for their future, and, this decision depended largely on their parents and environment. For the student respondents who had come to university from school, life so far has been ‘kind of set-out’ (41 Student), in the sense that they had progressed through the education system to gain qualifications for university. For their peer group it was normal still to be in education or training at the age of 18.

The lasting effects of early influences were particularly striking among the senior learners, because they were much further removed in years from their childhood. Many related stories about parental influence and also about teachers who taught them at least 50 years ago and had affected them for better or worse. For example a senior learner recalled one of her teachers;

I hated primary school – the teacher in 3rd or 4th year juniors [for ages 9–11] I hated her she was not a nice woman….. I passed to go to the grammar school and it shocked her. She made a derogatory comment – may not have been directed at me but felt it was- about some who should have passed and didn’t and some passing who should not have done…… I always vowed I would never be like that when I was teaching….(11 Senior Learner).

Those who related negative influences presented themselves as active in response, not necessarily at the time but later in their lives. For example a student whose mother had died wrote that ‘it made me more independent’ and another student who was bullied at school wrote that ‘it made me stronger’. The adult had to deal with all the influences (negative or positive) and take control.

Theme 2: Nature and nurture interact

While respondents’ view of themselves and of a child adopted at birth assigned greater influence to environment this did not mean that they held a simplistic model of, for example 60:40 nurture to nature. In this one question when they were asked to choose one or other as the major influence, almost all chose nurture, as many social scientists might do. However, in open questions and comments more complex interactive models were expressed. Environment/nurture can affect genes/nature and vice versa. No one used the term epigenetics but responses referred to the possibility of environmental influences affecting gene expression, for example;

People with certain predispositions (e.g. to violence) are affected by society, and society affects how their genes are expressed (40 Student).

An older respondent reflects on personal experience of child rearing and asks whether nurture is influenced by nature;

I think the nature nurture debate is very interesting. In my family I can see where my children have their own natures that have developed despite being brought up in the same family with the same boundaries etc. However, as a parent did I alter how I nurture them to take into account their nature? (14 Senior Learner).

This quotation illustrates the inseparability of nature and nurture. The child is developing within the family and the parent is developing parenting strategies informed by previous experiences and by other influences including the reactions of the children.

It was obvious to respondents that both genetic and environmental factors impact on everyone (although the role of genes is not yet understood) and it will be harder for some than for others to behave well because of their genes and environment. These people may need different treatment or extra help if they have committed violent and aggressive crimes but that does not excuse their behaviour. Only in exceptional cases, like insanity, can a young person or adult be said to have no choice but to act in a particular way. It is important that people are seen as responsible while also giving them the help they need. In these two comments the treatment for environmental problems and ‘biology’ are similar; the individual can be helped to modify his/her behaviour.

No, [nature and nurture] both play a part, but they can’t be the explanation for everything. Some people grow up in broken homes and get treated appallingly- yet they seem to understand right + wrong and accept responsibility for their actions. There are too many excuses and we never solve any problems, just make them harder to resolve.......I think if you are sane and you know right from wrong you need to suffer the consequences if you’ve committed a crime, but I do appreciate you may need help psychologically if you have anger issues, for example. If we constantly find reasons to diminish blame from people who have committed heinous acts of crime more people will think they can get away with it and it will cause more harm than good (78 Student). Some say you can’t fight your biology, but there are social factors that can stop bad behaviour like learned restraint (72 Student).

The desire to leave a space for individual agency may be linked to the finding that emphasising nature, but never nurture, could be dangerous. It is clear that as children grow up they can exercise more control over their environment, although some have more control and choices than others. On the other hand, whatever the individual is born with (genes and nature) is, or seems to be, less malleable which could lead to different criminal justice policies and different social perceptions of the criminal.

Theme 3: Emphasising nature (but never nurture) can be dangerous for society as a whole as well as for the criminal and victims

The question asked was whether it mattered ‘for individuals or society’ if either nature or nurture was seen as most important in explaining problem behavior. The two most popular answers were that both nature and nurture were needed to explain behaviour, or, that nurture was more important and that there were dangers in emphasising nature. No one in the sample regarded an emphasis on nurture as dangerous or detrimental to the individual or society. On the contrary, emphasising nurture was thought more likely to lead to non-punitive treatment of offenders. There would be attempts to alter future behaviour through improved education and parenting and spreading of knowledge in society about the impact nurture has on young people. Society as a whole would share the blame rather than the individual. As a student put it; ‘society as a whole [would be] open for criticism’ (55 S). An emphasis on nurture was therefore seen as more likely to lead to understanding of problem behaviours and effective treatment, however, the individuals were still to be held responsible for their behaviour.

In contrast there was a mistrust of nature/genetic explanations that again centred on the practical consequences for individuals. It would affect the way criminals were treated by others but could also change their view of themselves. Behaviour would be seen as unchangeable, out of the control of the individual or social action. As a consequence, individual accountability might be removed. The idea that individuals must normally be held responsible for their actions was constantly emphasised (Levitt, 2013 ).

It does [matter] because [if nurture is emphasised] people will care, parent and look after and raise people with more care. However if it’s proven it is nature, then people may lose the will to live (60 Student).

Several SLs referred to the examination at the end of primary education (the ‘eleven plus’) when explaining why they emphasised environment/nurture rather than nature, or, in this case, innate intelligence. The ‘eleven plus’ examination was used to decide which children would be offered a place at an academically selective grammar school and was based on the idea that intelligence, and future academic achievement, could be accurately measured and predicted at the age of 10 or 11.

‘The 11+ was a nature thing. I did the 11+ − it had an effect. Saying children not going to improve or change. Very embedded in the whole idea of nature – it can’t really be true’ (8 Senior Learner).

An emphasis on nature has practical detrimental consequences for individuals. Their status is fixed, for example as ‘not academic’ or ‘born evil’ and suggests, to them and to others, that their ‘nature’ is unchangeable or very difficult to change by individual or social action.

Yes, [it matters] hugely as position of blame is dependent on whether a person chose to do what they did .....nature suggests no control (35 Student).

Those who thought an emphasis on nature meant people were irredeemable either gave that as a reason not to emphasise nature or to suggest that in fact ‘defects’ of nature could be overcome, as in this comment by a student emphasising the power of education;

Yes it is very important because it helps to understand if people are reformable (nurture) or irredeemable (nature). I believe we are determined by our education and thus with the proper help we can change. In the case of people with major biological defects, education is still a way to get over these obstacles and society should be ready to help these people (38 Student).

It might be thought that offenders themselves would embrace a genetic explanation of their behaviour if this was interpreted, as the respondents feared, as meaning they were not responsible for their crimes. However, a small study of juvenile offenders in the Netherlands found that they gave social explanations of their crimes and most rejected the idea that biology might be a factor. They committed a crime for a specific purpose like to get money or to impress others or they gave environmental reasons such as a deprived background or peer pressure or explained their offences were due to psychological conditions brought on by the use of alcohol and soft drugs (Horstkötter et al., 2012 , p.291). Whether they gave goal directed or environmental reasons ‘most of them also state that they had a choice and that it was their choice to commit the crime’ (ibid p.292). As one young offender said in interview;

In the end the person makes the choice himself… The choices I have made also had a share in my past. But in the end I am the one who has made these choices (ibid).

Genes and environment

Respondents were at ease with the language of nature and nurture which was only used in the introduction to the questionnaire or interview. They readily equated genes with nature and nurture with all sorts of environmental influences. There was an acknowledgement that our understanding of environmental factors is greater than our understanding of genetics but that that would change. Older respondents were more likely to be concerned about such a change.

They're going to be doing a lot more with genetics. Influences policy profoundly and people have to be very careful. It worries me that seen to be [more determining]. The complexities don’t get looked at. If you emphasise environment it is safer from a policy point of view because given that most people don’t know what they are talking about it is safer to see the person as redeemable than to come down on the side of genetics and write people off (3 Senior Learner).

This quotation is typical in its view that nature/genes are seen as determining even though the influences on behaviour are, in reality, complex. Like the studies quoted at the beginning of the article respondents often acknowledged the complexities as nature and nurture interact but separated them when explaining the causes of specific behaviours. Students were less likely to be fearful of genetic explanations of behaviour despite their academic interest in social science. However, the hypothesis that young people might be more likely to be interested in genetic explanations for behaviour was not shown in this small study. The senior learners were more likely to refer to reading on genes and display knowledge of genetics. Older respondents and their relatives more often echoed the sociologists’ concerns about behavioural genetics discussed by Bearman earlier (Bearman, 2008 ). For those who feared the practical consequences of genetic explanations, like the respondent quoted above, ‘it is safer’ to keep away from them.

Some respondents in all age groups were prepared for advances in genetics to change their understanding of behaviour and prepared for current views of genes/nature as more basic, fixed and unchanging to change too. One of the youngest relatives, in her 20s, emphasised our incomplete knowledge of genetic influences on behaviour as a reason for focussing on nurture ‘at present’;

It is very tricky as we cannot see genes and I am not sure that I totally trust the idea of blaming genes for violent behaviour- maybe the person has a gene for passive behaviour as well. …….In any case we can change nurture but at present we cannot change nature so let’s do one thing at a time (20 Relative).

As respondents in this small study grappled with explanations for their own and others’ behaviour they focussed on the practical consequences leading to a greater concern over explanations based on nature than the more familiar ones based on a complex web of environmental factors. Whereas academic researchers approach the debate from their disciplinary perspectives which may or may not engage with practical and policy issues, the key issue for the public was what sort of explanations of behaviour will lead to the best outcomes for all concerned.

1 Behavioural epigenetic research has indicated that life experiences can affect gene expression. While controversial the research suggests the possibility of further complications for the nature-nurture relationship as nurture may be said to shape nature (Buchen, 2010 Powledge, 2011 ). 2 Bearman op cit iv. The ESRC Cambridge Network Social Contexts of Pathways into Crime (SCoPiC) promoted multidisciplinary research into the causes of crime and included the E risk longitudinal twin study led by Terri Moffitt which investigated how genetic and environmental factors shape children's disruptive behaviour http://www.scopic.ac.uk Accessed 3 Sep 2013. 3 Violent and antisocial behaviour in this longitudinal study was correlated with a common genetic trait (low expression of MAOA) only where the person was severely maltreated in childhood. Behaviour was measured on 4 outcomes; diagnoses of conduct disorder, psychological tests of aggression and anti-social personality disorder and convictions for violent crime. Caspi et al. 2002 (supplementary material). 4 This initial warm-up question implied that the influences of nature and nurture could be separated and quantified as in common usage both in academic and popular discourses. As discussed respondents were able to express their views more fully (and with more complexity) in the subsequent open questions.

Acknowledgement

The support of the Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC) is gratefully acknowledged. This work was part of the Research Programme of the ESRC Genomics Network at Cesagen (ESRC Centre for Economic and Social Aspects of Genomics).

Competing interests

The author declares that she has no competing interests.

  • Anderson P, Butcher KF, Schanzenbach DW. Childhood obesity and disadvantage: is nature trumping nurture? NBER working paper No. 13479. Cambridge MA: National Bureau of Economic Research; 2007. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bearman P. Exploring genetics and social structure. American Journal of Sociology. 2008; 114 (Suppl;S1):v–x. doi: 10.1086/596596. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brescianini S, Volzone A, Fagnani C, Patriarca V, Grimaldi V, Lanni R, Serino L, Mastroiacovo P, Stazi MA. Genetic and environmental factors shape infant sleep patterns: a study of 18-month-old twins. Pediatrics. 2011; 127 (5):1296–1302. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-0858. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Buchen L. In their nurture – can epigenetics underlie the enduring effects for a mother’s love. Nature. 2010; 467 :146–148. doi: 10.1038/467146a. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Campbell E, Ross LF. Attitudes of healthcare professionals and parents regarding genetic testing for violent traits in childhood. Journal of Medical Ethics. 2004; 30 :580–586. doi: 10.1136/jme.2003.005389. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Caspi A, et al. Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science. 2002; 2 (5582):851–854. doi: 10.1126/science.1072290. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Claassen L, Henneman L, De Vet R, Knol D, Marteau T, Timmermans D. Fatalistic responses to different types of genetic risk information: Exploring the role of Self-Malleability. Psychology and Health. 2010; 25 (2):183–196. doi: 10.1080/08870440802460434. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Condit CM. When do people deploy genetic determinism? A review pointing to te need for multi-Factorial theories of public utilization of scientific discourses. Sociology Compass. 2010; 5 (7):618–635. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9020.2011.00385.x. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cooper JA, Walsh A, Ellis L. Is criminology moving toward a paradigm shift? Evidence from a Survey of the American Society of Criminology Journal of Criminal Justice Education. 2010; 21 (3):332–347. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Craddock N. Horses for courses: the need for pragmatism and realism as well as balance and caution. A commentary on Angel. Social Science and Medicine. 2011; 73 :636–638. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.06.049. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • DeLisa M, Wright JP, Vaughn MG, Beaver KM. Nature and nurture by definition means both: a response to males. Journal of Adolescent Research. 2010; 25 (1):24–30. doi: 10.1177/0743558409353063. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Denno DW. Behavioral genetics evidence in criminal cases: 1994–2007. In: Farahany NA, editor. The impact of behavioral sciences on criminal law. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2009. pp. 317–354. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dixon M. Brave new choices: behavioural genetics and public policy. London: IPPR; 2005. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Farahany NA, Coleman JE., Jr Genetics and responsibility: to know the criminal from the Crime. Law and Contemporary Problems. 2006; 69 :115–162. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fotaki M. Agency versus structure or nature versus nurture: when the new twist on an old debate is not that new after all: a commentary on Angel. Social Science & Medicine. 2011; 73 (5):639–642. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.06.044. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Giddens A. Sociology. Cambridge: Polity Press; 2009. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Haralambos M, Holborn M. Sociology themes and perspectives. London: Collins Educational; 2004. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Horstkötter D, Berghmans R, de Ruiter C, Krumeich A, de Wert G. “We are also normal human beings, you know”. Views and attitudes of juvenile delinquents on antisocial behaviour, neurobiology and prevention. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry. 2012; 35 :289–297. doi: 10.1016/j.ijlp.2012.04.006. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Keller EF. The mirage of a space between nature and nurture. Durham & London: Duke University Press; 2010. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lea R, Chambers G. Monamine oxidase, addiction and the ‘warrior’ gene hypothesis. The New Zealand Medical Journal. 2007; 120 :1250. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Levitt M. Genes, environment and responsibility for violent behaviour: ‘Whatever genes one has it is preferable that you are prevented from going around stabbing people’ New Genetics and Society. 2013; 32 (1):4–17. doi: 10.1080/14636778.2012.699352. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Levitt M, Pieri E. “It could just be an additional test couldn’t it?” Genetic testing for susceptibility to aggression and violence. New Genetics and Society. 2009; 28 (2):189–200. doi: 10.1080/14636770902901629. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McCrae RR, Costa PT, Jr, Ostendorf F, Angleitner A, Hřebíčková M, Avia MD, Sanz J, Sánchez-Bernardos ML, Kusdil ME, Woodfield R, Saunders PR, Smith PB. Nature over nurture: temperament, personality and life span development. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2000; 78 (1):173–86. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.173. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nuffield Council on Bioethics . Genetics and human behaviour: the ethical context. London: Nuffield Council on Bioethics; 2002. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Parens E, Chapman ER, N. Press N, editors. Wrestling with behavioural genetics. Science, ethics and public conversation. Baltimore: The John Hopkins Press; 2006. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Powledge TM. Behavioral Epigenetics: How nurture shapes nature. Biosciences. 2011; 61 :588–592. doi: 10.1525/bio.2011.61.8.4. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Traynor BJ, Singleton AB. Nature versus nurture: death of a dogma, and the road ahead. Neuron. 2010; 68 :196–200. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.10.002. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Walsh A. Biology and criminality. The biosocial synthesis. Oxon: Routledge; 2009. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Search Search
  • CN (Chinese)
  • DE (German)
  • ES (Spanish)
  • FR (Français)
  • JP (Japanese)
  • Open Research
  • Booksellers
  • Peer Reviewers
  • Springer Nature Group ↗
  • Publish an article
  • Roles and responsibilities
  • Signing your contract
  • Writing your manuscript
  • Submitting your manuscript
  • Producing your book
  • Promoting your book
  • Submit your book idea
  • Manuscript guidelines
  • Book author services
  • Publish a book
  • Publish conference proceedings

LaTeX Author Support

  • Journal authors ↓
  • Book authors ↓

Journal Authors

We recommend that you use the Springer Nature LaTeX authoring template, which you can download as a .zip or access via Overleaf . This template takes a ‘content first’ approach with minimal formatting. Instead it is designed to promote editorial policy best practice, and move seamlessly through editorial and production systems.

Preparing your research

We understand that many researchers prefer to use LaTeX when preparing a manuscript for submission, but that it can sometimes be difficult to navigate. That’s why we’re providing this comprehensive set of LaTeX resources to help simplify the process. 

To help avoid any errors: 

  • Please do not use any custom fonts
  • Make sure you convert special characters, including diacritics such as ä/ö/ü, into the appropriate TeX code, like \"a, \"o, \"u

Submitting your research

We know from feedback that uploading source files for submission can be time consuming and, if those files fail to convert, the reasons are not always easy to identify.

To help with your submission:

  • Please check for errors in your local compilation before uploading your files to the submission system
  • Please fix any errors before you try to submit your paper, otherwise it will most likely fail to compile

If you are submitting to Snapp your files must be compressed in .zip format. If you are submitting to Editorial Manager and your source files fail to convert, you will be provided with an error log file to help you identify the source of the error, for example: ! LaTeX Error: File ‘trackchanges.sty’ not found. If you are submitting to eJP using the Springer Nature LaTeX template you will need to use the pdflatex option in the preamble to allow PDF compilation.

Books authors

Regardless of the authoring software you chose to prepare your book manuscript it is important that your files are prepared in line with the guidelines for book authors .

For LaTeX users, Springer Nature provides templates for LaTeX users that help structure the manuscript, e.g., define the heading hierarchy. Predefined style formats are available for all the necessary structures that are supposed to be part of the manuscript.

Springer Conference Proceedings

Guidelines and technical instructions are provided for the preparation of contributions to one of the Springer Conference Proceedings. For contributions to one of the Computer Science Proceedings in particular this includes: The LaTeX2e Proceedings Templates (zip) for download The LaTeX2e Proceedings Template in Overleaf

Here we have answered some of the most common questions that arise at the point of submitting an article in LaTeX. If your question relates specifically to the Springer Nature LaTeX authoring template, please consult the User Manual provided in the .zip package.

How do I identify which submission system I’m using?

Editorial Manager

  • Can be identified by the prefix “https://www.editorialmanager.com” in the url, or the banner “em Editorial Manager” at the top of the page after the journal name. 
  • Compiles files to PDF via TexLive 2018.
  • Can be identified by the prefix “https://mts-” in the url, the banner “manuscripttrackingsystem” at the top of the page, or the logo eJournalPress at the bottom of the page. Typically any submissions to a Nature-branded or Nature Research journal (including npjs, Scientific Data, and Communications journals) are made via eJP. 
  • Compiles files to PDF via TexLive 2017.

Springer Nature Article Processing Platform

  • Can be identified by the prefix “https://submission.nature.com/” in the url, or the Springer Nature logo at the top of the page.
  • Compiles files to PDF via TexLive 2021.

How should I upload my bibliography?

Either .bbl or .bib files are accepted and should be uploaded as “Manuscript” item types on the file upload screen of Editorial Manager. In both cases the correct bibliography style (.bst) file should also be uploaded and the chosen bibliography file should be referenced within the manuscript (.tex) file. Please note a .bbl file should only be uploaded if referenced (via “import”) in the main manuscript (.tex) file. 

Bibliography files should be uploaded as a “related manuscript file”.  If submitting with .bib the correct bibliography style (.bst) file must also be present.

For both submission systems the most reliable way to submit references without error is to paste the content of a bibliography file (.bbl) into the main .tex document in place of a \bibliography{…} command. 

Why does the compiled PDF display "??" in place of cited references?

If references are uploaded according to the guidance above and “??” continues to appear in place of in-line citations; the bibliography file may still be missing or corrupted.

One or more files may be missing or incorrectly declared. Check that: -  If the .tex file references a .bib file, ensure that the .bib file is uploaded (as a “Manuscript” item type). -  If a .bbl file has been uploaded, that it is referenced (via “import”) in the .tex manuscript. -  A .bib file is included with the submission. -  The correct bibliography style (.bst) file is present.

Where square boxes or other inline text errors are present it may indicate that special characters have been used. All special characters must be converted into the appropriate TeX code, like \"a, \"o, \"u.

If submitting with .bib the correct bibliography style (.bst) file must be present. -  Upload .bib and .bst as ‘Related Manuscript Files’ -  If a .bbl file is not being recognised please check that the bibliography explicitly contains references as bibitems. This can be achieved by copying the contents (individual bibitems) from the bibliography file into the bibliography environment within the main .tex file. -  Double check that the argument to the relevant bibitem matches the citation in the text before submitting. 

If you continue to have problems after trying these tips then the most reliable way to submit references without error is to paste the content of a bibliography file (.bbl) into the main .tex document in place of a \bibliography{…} command. 

Why are my figures missing?

Figures will be missing from the compiled PDF if: 

-  Subfolders (or subdirectories) have been used. All files should be included in a single directory when uploading the submission. -  If the full path to the image in the “\includegraphics” command is included  [e.g.  \includegraphics{C:\Users\username\Pictures\figure_1.jpg}].  To correct this you should use only the local path, e.g. \includegraphics{figure_1.jpg}.  -  The wrong option for the graphicx package is used. When compiling in pdflatex use graphicxs without the dvips option \usepackage{graphicx}, not \usepackage[dvips]{graphicx}.  

Why do special characters not appear correctly?

-  Custom fonts are not supported by either submission system. 

-  Diacritics, such as German umlauts like ä/ö/ü should be converted into the appropriate TeX code, like \"a, \"o, \"u. Eszett (SS/ß) should be \"s (upper case) or \“z (lower case). If that does not work an alternative code option would be to add brackets: {\"a}, {\"o}, {\"u}, { \"s}, { \"z}.

Which journals can I use the Springer Nature LaTeX authoring template for?

The Springer Nature LaTeX authoring template can be used to prepare your journal article submission to any Springer Nature journal inclusive of Springer, Nature Portfolio, and BMC. The template takes a ‘content first’ approach with minimal formatting. It is designed to promote editorial policy best practice, and move more seamlessly through editorial and production systems. The template is not a comprehensive ‘Guide to Authors’ so you must check journal-level instructions for authors for any specific formatting or style requirements when preparing your manuscript. 

Authors preparing submissions for one of the Springer Conference Proceedings, including Lecture Notes in Computer Science should continue to refer to the guidelines and technical instructions provided for the preparation of conference proceedings.

Still having problems? If you cannot find the answer you need here, or in the FAQs, please contact the editorial office for the journal you are submitting to in the first instance. To help them help you please try and include the following information with your query:

  • When the problem occurred 
  • Any error messages you received 
  • Do your LaTeX files compile without error locally?

L_brandstrip_bmc

  • Tools & Services
  • Account Development
  • Sales and account contacts
  • Professional
  • Press office
  • Locations & Contact

We are a world leading research, educational and professional publisher. Visit our main website for more information.

  • © 2024 Springer Nature
  • General terms and conditions
  • Your US State Privacy Rights
  • Your Privacy Choices / Manage Cookies
  • Accessibility
  • Legal notice
  • Help us to improve this site, send feedback.
  • Small Language Model
  • Computer Vision
  • Federated Learning
  • Reinforcement Learning
  • Natural Language Processing
  • New Releases
  • 🧩 AI Webinars
  • Promotion ⮕ 1.5M+ AI Readers

Logo

The training of FLAMe involves a meticulous process of data collection and standardization. The research team curated human evaluations from previous studies, focusing on tasks such as machine translation quality and AI assistant instruction. This extensive dataset was then reformatted into a unified text-to-text format, where each quality assessment task was converted into input-target pairs. The inputs include task-specific contexts, while the targets contain expected human evaluations. By training on this large and diverse dataset, FLAMe learns robust patterns of human judgment, minimizing the impact of noisy or low-quality data. The FLAMe-RM variant, specifically fine-tuned for reward modeling evaluation, exemplifies this methodology’s effectiveness. Fine-tuned for only 50 steps on a mixture of four datasets covering chat, reasoning, and safety, FLAMe-RM demonstrates significant improvements in performance.

The performance of FLAMe is noteworthy across various benchmarks. The FLAMe-RM-24B model, a variant fine-tuned for reward modeling evaluation, achieved an accuracy of 87.8% on RewardBench, surpassing both GPT-4-0125 (85.9%) and GPT-4o (84.7%). On the CoBBLEr bias benchmark, FLAMe exhibits significantly lower bias compared to other autorater models. In addition to RewardBench, FLAMe’s performance is strong on other benchmarks. The FLAMe models outperform existing LLMs on 8 out of 12 automated evaluation benchmarks, covering 53 quality assessment tasks. This includes tasks such as summary comparisons, helpfulness evaluations, and factual accuracy assessments. The results demonstrate FLAMe’s broad applicability and robust performance across diverse evaluation scenarios.

nature.com research paper

FLAMe-Opt-RM, a computationally efficient variant, optimizes the multitask mixture for reward modeling evaluation using a novel tail-patch fine-tuning strategy. This method fine-tunes the initial instruction-tuned PaLM-2-24B checkpoint on an optimized mixture for 5000 steps, achieving competitive RewardBench performance with approximately 25 times fewer training data points. The research highlights that longer training and additional fine-tuning can improve performance, suggesting that FLAMe-Opt-RM is a versatile and efficient model.

To conclude, the research highlights the importance of reliable and efficient evaluation methods for LLMs. FLAMe offers a robust solution by leveraging standardized human evaluations, demonstrating significant improvements in performance and bias reduction. This advancement is poised to enhance the development and deployment of AI technologies. The FLAMe family of models, developed by a collaborative team from Google DeepMind, Google, and UMass Amherst, represents a significant step forward in evaluating large language models, ensuring their outputs are reliable, unbiased, and of high quality.

Check out the Paper. All credit for this research goes to the researchers of this project. Also, don’t forget to follow us on  Twitter . 

Join our  Telegram Channel and  LinkedIn Gr oup .

If you like our work, you will love our  newsletter..

Don’t Forget to join our  46k+ ML SubReddit

nature.com research paper

Nikhil is an intern consultant at Marktechpost. He is pursuing an integrated dual degree in Materials at the Indian Institute of Technology, Kharagpur. Nikhil is an AI/ML enthusiast who is always researching applications in fields like biomaterials and biomedical science. With a strong background in Material Science, he is exploring new advancements and creating opportunities to contribute.

  • MMLongBench-Doc: A Comprehensive Benchmark for Evaluating Long-Context Document Understanding in Large Vision-Language Models
  • This AI Paper Introduces TelecomGPT: A Domain-Specific Large Language Model for Enhanced Performance in Telecommunication Tasks
  • This AI Paper by Prime Intellect Introduces OpenDiLoCo: An Open-Source Framework for Globally Distributed Low-Communication Training
  • This AI Paper Introduces GAVEL: A System Combining Large Language Models and Evolutionary Algorithms for Creative Game Design

RELATED ARTICLES MORE FROM AUTHOR

Muse: a comprehensive ai framework for evaluating machine unlearning in language models, efficient quantization-aware training (efficientqat): a novel machine learning quantization technique for compressing llms, google research presents a novel ai method for genetic discovery that can harness hidden information in high-dimensional clinical data, researchers from the university of auckland introduced chatlogic: enhancing multi-step reasoning in large language models with over 50% accuracy improvement in complex tasks, pinokio 2.0: a new pinokio browser version that lets you locally install, run, and automate any ai on your computer, needlebench: a customizable dataset framework that includes tasks for evaluating the bilingual long-context capabilities of llms across multiple length intervals, google research presents a novel ai method for genetic discovery that can harness hidden..., researchers from the university of auckland introduced chatlogic: enhancing multi-step reasoning in large language..., pinokio 2.0: a new pinokio browser version that lets you locally install, run, and automate....

  • AI Magazine
  • Privacy & TC
  • Cookie Policy
  • 🐝 Partnership and Promotion

🐝 FREE AI Courses on RAG + Deployment of an Healthcare AI App + LangChain Colab Notebook all included

Thank You 🙌

Privacy Overview

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals

Nature and the Nature research journals

As part of the Nature Portfolio, Nature  has spent the last 150 years championing and supporting scientific work. Alongside our Nature research journals, we continue that mission in partnering with researchers from across the globe to bring their most significant discoveries to the widest possible audience. 

We identify and publish primary research that makes major advances in a field

nature.com research paper

Articles published in Nature and the Nature research journals have contributed to some of the world’s most notable scientific advances. From the discovery of radioactive decay, to Watson and Crick’s seminal work on DNA, to Dolly the sheep and cloning, read more how our articles have played a critical role in advancing their fields and helping to address global societal challenges.

Rigorous manuscript development

Our editors assess and select the most important research to publish, partnering with authors and reviewers to deliver significant improvements to manuscripts through our rigorous and intensive review process.

Systematic reproducibility checks 

In 2013 we introduced a mandatory reproducibility checklist requiring detailed reporting of analytical and experimental design as well as statistical analyses. 83% of surveyed Nature authors felt our checklist had significantly improved reporting of statistics within papers published in Nature .

Innovation in peer review and transparency

Nature and Nature research journals conduct ethics and biosecurity peer review, peer review of code and data, and offer transparent peer review, publishing peer reviewer comments alongside the paper should the authors wish to do so.

Open data and code

We support communities by ensuring deposition of data and code alongside the research article to maximise transparency and reuse. Our active encouragement of data deposition has boosted data availability associated with published papers from around 50% to over 80% in some disciplines.

We encourage authors to submit protocols to the Protocols Exchange – an open sharing platform – these provide valuable detailed reporting on the analytical and experimental design as well as statistical analyses.

Critical assessment

Our highly qualified specialist editors will provide initial assessment ahead of selecting the most relevant peer reviewers for each research article, then work through revisions with our authors, to ensure the final research articles published will achieve the greatest significance.

“It was a unique situation, during coronacrisis, that our manuscript was handled by three editors at Nature . We really appreciated direct, timely and professional communication from all editors. They all had detailed knowledge of our manuscript and steered the editorial process very professionally. The editors were focused on critical aspects of the review process with special attention to technical issues raised by the reviewers. Overall, the editorial and review process motivated us to further upgrade and improve our manuscript.” Professor, Germany

“From the editorial staff to the productions [sic] teams have always conducted themselves in the most professional manner. I use the term 'most' literally because the Nature series journals' personnel are more professional than any other journal in which I have had work rejected or published.” Associate Professor, Canada

“As we always find with publishing in Nature the entire process is undertaken to the highest standards. There is also a very nice personal touch in the way all the editors and publication processing team interacts with the authors.” Associate Professor, UK

nature.com research paper

In addition, there are many dedicated people behind every paper we publish , including copy editors who check and improve language for clarity and scientific accuracy; art illustrators who redraw schemes and diagrams so that they are clear and can be used for teaching, talks and presentations; production staff who ensure the paper is formatted, published, and delivered to all relevant manuscript repositories; and a dedicated press team who ensure the research is explained and disseminated to the widest possible audience across society and the world.

Amplifying our authors’ work

nature.com research paper

Global reach and engagement

We use our global reach and the nature.com platform to engage with research communities beyond the immediate fields of our author’s work - aiding interdisciplinarity and collaboration and providing researchers with access to  articles that they didn’t realise would be of interest to them. Nature.com reaches more than 8 million visitors each month.

Making research part of the wider conversation

There were more than 100,000 news stories on research published in our journals in 2019. Our dedicated international press teams worked alongside authors to reach a wide range of audiences across the globe, including other researchers, policy makers and the general public with news of their research, reported clearly and accurately.

Enhanced citations

On average, papers in the Nature and Nature research journals are cited around 12 times more than papers published in other journals, and downloaded around 34 times more.

Finding the right home for your work at the Nature Portfolio and Springer Nature

Our editorial teams work with authors to find the best home for all submitted manuscripts from our wider family of journals, both at the Nature Portfolio and more widely across our other Springer Nature imprints, including Springer, BMC and Palgrave Macmillan. Whether it is your first paper, or the one that will make your career, there’s a place for your research in our journals.

We launch new journals that reflect the needs of our community and society 

We are fascinated by the evolution of science, and as new disciplines emerge and established fields intersect, we are driven to provide a platform to champion the questions, solutions and discussions this evolution brings. Our primary goal is to produce journals that the scientific community values, both as readers and authors, launching new journals such as Nature Ecology & Evolution to meet the needs of the research community. 

We also aim to create innovative journals that span both the natural and social sciences to help researchers address some of the biggest challenges that are facing global society, such as Nature Climate Change , Nature Human Behaviour and Nature Sustainability .

Recognising the growing importance of open research to advancing discovery, the Nature Portfolio also includes a flourishing collection of fully open access journals, both subject specific, such as the Communications journals and multidisciplinary such as Nature Communications and Scientific Reports . From 2021, every journal in the Nature Portfolio will include an open access option for all primary research.

Ultimately, if your work is scientifically robust, there is a home for your research within our family of journals.

Take a look at our  Inside Nature  infographic. 

Ready to publish?

Find out more in our information for authors .

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

nature.com research paper

IMAGES

  1. 😀 Research paper on plants. Sample Scientific Research Paper. 2019-02-26

    nature.com research paper

  2. (PDF) The Nature of Research

    nature.com research paper

  3. Research Papers in Environmental Science Template

    nature.com research paper

  4. Nature research journals Template

    nature.com research paper

  5. (PDF) How to Write Ecology Research Papers

    nature.com research paper

  6. Reflection Paper about nature's patterns

    nature.com research paper

VIDEO

  1. Story of Hazrat Younus A S

  2. Story of Hazrat Loot A.S

  3. Al Isra wal Mi'raj

  4. Final Episode of Hazrat Nooh A.S Story

  5. Ashaab ul Ukhdood

  6. Ashaab e Kahaf

COMMENTS

  1. Research articles

    A holistic platform for accelerating sorbent-based carbon capture. A framework that integrates materials, process design, techno-economics and life-cycle assessment can be used to accelerate the ...

  2. Browse Articles

    Browse the archive of articles on Nature. ... Research Highlight 19 Jul 2024. ... This tiny solar-powered flyer weighs less than a paper plane.

  3. Nature

    First published in 1869, Nature is the world's leading multidisciplinary science journal. Nature publishes the finest peer-reviewed research that drives ground-breaking discovery, and is read by ...

  4. Nature (journal)

    Nature is a British weekly scientific journal founded and based in London, England.As a multidisciplinary publication, Nature features peer-reviewed research from a variety of academic disciplines, mainly in science and technology. It has core editorial offices across the United States, continental Europe, and Asia under the international scientific publishing company Springer Nature.

  5. Research articles

    Sustained underweight in rural areas and emergence of overweight in urban Ethiopian women: a multivariate analysis of EDHS data 2000-2016. Amare Abera Tareke. Addis Alem. Taddese Alemu Zerfu. Article Open Access 19 Jul 2024.

  6. Writing a Research Paper: 2nd Edition course

    The benefits of this new 2nd edition. Restructured content for a better learning experience. Enriched content with extensive real-world examples. Bite-size lessons on each topic to fit busy schedules. Strategies to apply narrative tools when writing research papers. Detailed examples for explaining concepts, taken from real papers where possible.

  7. Nature Research journals

    Reviews, News & Opinion content in the Nature Portfolio journals, encompassing news, views, analysis, opinions, and reviews, fuels debate and drives change within research communities and beyond. This content educates and inspires future scientists, supports global collaboration, and addresses the world's greatest challenges. It is widely ...

  8. Writing a Research Paper course

    Writing a Research Paper. For students and researchers in the natural sciences who are new to scientific writing or wish to improve the quality of their written output. Taught by 17 Nature Portfolio journal Editors. 4.5 hours of learning. 15-minute lessons.

  9. Training for researchers & scientists

    Professional development training for researchers — via online courses and workshops. Grow your research skills, confidence and career. Learn from renowned international experts including Nature Portfolio journal Editors. Find training to support you across the research lifecycle.

  10. Pathogenicity and transmissibility of bovine H5N1 influenza virus

    Nature - Pathogenicity and transmissibility of bovine H5N1 influenza virus. ... Influenza Research Institute, Dept. of Pathobiological Sciences, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, USA ...

  11. Find submission status of your article / manuscript

    You can also access it via the starting page of your Springer Nature Profile where you will need to login and navigate to "Your Research". Here, you will see the segment "Track the progress of your submitted articles", where you will see the status of your latest submission. Underneath, you will see "Track all submissions" where you will find a ...

  12. Scientists discover a cause of lupus and a possible way to reverse it

    In a study published in Nature on July 10, the scientists report a new pathway that drives disease in lupus. There are disease-associated changes in multiple molecules in the blood of patients with lupus. Ultimately, these changes lead to insufficient activation of a pathway controlled by the aryl hydrocarbon receptor (AHR), which regulates cells' response to environmental pollutants ...

  13. Journals

    Explore the leading weekly interdisciplinary science journal and discover Nature Portfolio journals which publish original research across the life, physical and clinical sciences.. Our academic journals are prestigious titles in a broad range of specialties across 42 journals.Scientific American is the leading source and authority for science, technology information and policy for a general ...

  14. Behind the Paper

    The real stories behind the latest research papers, from conception to publication, the highs and the lows. Protein Structure Mapping: Unveiling the Mechanism of Diazirine Photo-reactions. Nature Communications. Behind the Paper.

  15. Nature Contact and Human Health: A Research Agenda

    We propose a research agenda on nature contact and health, identifying principal domains of research and key questions that, if answered, would provide the basis for evidence-based public health interventions. Discussion: We identify research questions in seven domains: a) mechanistic biomedical studies; b) exposure science; c) epidemiology of ...

  16. Publishing a Research Paper course

    Publishing a Research Paper. For researchers in the natural sciences who are new to publishing or wish to refresh their skills. Taught by 20 Nature Portfolio journal experts. 5.5 hours of learning. 15-minute lessons. 8-module course with certificate.

  17. The Human-Nature Relationship and Its Impact on Health: A Critical

    Evolutionary Biology. Evolutionary biology is a branch of research that shortly followed Darwin's Theory of Evolution.It concerns the adaptive nature of variation in all animal and plant life, shaped by genetic architecture and developmental processes over time and space ().Since its emergence over a century ago, the field has made some significant advances in scientific knowledge, but with ...

  18. A Recent Study Found Toxic Metals in Tampons. Here's What to Know

    A new study about tampons has sparked concern among consumers. The research, published in the journal Environment International, found evidence of 16 different metals in a wide range of tampons ...

  19. Research articles

    Now an ABC collagen mimetic heterotrimer has been designed that employs pairwise amino acid interactions, validated by X-ray crystallography, to promote composition- and register-specific assembly ...

  20. Home

    Welcome to the Springer Nature press site. The Springer Nature journals press office promotes research and other content published in the Nature Portfolio, BMC, Palgrave Macmillan and Springer titles in the Springer Nature portfolio. Our press site provides journalists with embargoed access to press releases, full-text PDFs of upcoming articles ...

  21. How do we study resilience? A systematic review

    This was most frequent when nature-based livelihood systems were considered, such as in Sapkota et al.'s paper on the resilience of community forestry in Nepal, which defines a community forestry system as 'the processes and outcomes of interactions in community forestry', the latter comprising the environment, society, and the various ...

  22. Scientific Writing & Publishing course

    Institutions are subscribing to our 'Scientific Writing and Publishing' courses to support their researchers in getting published. Explore the role of a peer reviewer and the foundations of a good peer review. One- and two-day workshops, available to institutions worldwide.

  23. Perceptions of nature, nurture and behaviour

    Trying to separate out nature and nurture as explanations for behaviour, as in classic genetic studies of twins and families, is now said to be both impossible and unproductive. In practice the nature-nurture model persists as a way of framing discussion on the causes of behaviour in genetic research papers, as well as in the media and lay debate.

  24. Toolkit: How to write a great paper

    A clear format will ensure that your research paper is understood by your readers. Follow: 1. Context — your introduction. 2. Content — your results. 3. Conclusion — your discussion. Plan ...

  25. Download the journal article template package ...

    We recommend that you use the Springer Nature LaTeX authoring template, which you can download as a .zip or access via Overleaf. This template takes a 'content first' approach with minimal formatting. Instead it is designed to promote editorial policy best practice, and move seamlessly through editorial and production systems.

  26. How to write your paper

    Writing for a Nature journal. Before writing a paper, authors are advised to visit the author information pages of the journal to which they wish to submit (see this link for a full list of Nature ...

  27. This AI Paper from Google AI Introduces FLAMe: A Foundational Large

    Evaluating large language models (LLMs) has become increasingly challenging due to their complexity and versatility. Ensuring the reliability and quality of these models' outputs is crucial for advancing AI technologies and applications. Researchers need help developing reliable evaluation methods to assess the accuracy and impartiality of LLMs' outputs, given human evaluations' subjective ...

  28. Nature and the Nature research journals

    On average, papers in the Nature and Nature research journals are cited around 12 times more than papers published in other journals, and downloaded around 34 times more.