Observation
The littlest linguists: new research on language development.
- Bilingualism
- Developmental Psychology
- Language Development
How do children learn language, and how is language related to other cognitive and social skills? For decades, the specialized field of developmental psycholinguistics has studied how children acquire language—or multiple languages—taking into account biological, neurological, and social factors that influence linguistic developments and, in turn, can play a role in how children learn and socialize. Here’s a look at recent research (2020–2021) on language development published in Psychological Science .
Preverbal Infants Discover Statistical Word Patterns at Similar Rates as Adults: Evidence From Neural Entrainment
Dawoon Choi, Laura J. Batterink, Alexis K. Black, Ken A. Paller, and Janet F. Werker (2020)
One of the first challenges faced by infants during language acquisition is identifying word boundaries in continuous speech. This neurological research suggests that even preverbal infants can learn statistical patterns in language, indicating that they may have the ability to segment words within continuous speech.
Using electroencephalogram measures to track infants’ ability to segment words, Choi and colleagues found that 6-month-olds’ neural processing increasingly synchronized with the newly learned words embedded in speech over the learning period in one session in the laboratory. Specifically, patterns of electrical activity in their brains increasingly aligned with sensory regularities associated with word boundaries. This synchronization was comparable to that seen among adults and predicted future ability to discriminate words.
These findings indicate that infants and adults may follow similar learning trajectories when tracking probabilities in speech, with both groups showing a logarithmic (rather than linear) increase in the synchronization of neural processing with frequent words. Moreover, speech segmentation appears to use neural mechanisms that emerge early in life and are maintained throughout adulthood.
Parents Fine-Tune Their Speech to Children’s Vocabulary Knowledge
Ashley Leung, Alexandra Tunkel, and Daniel Yurovsky (2021)
Children can acquire language rapidly, possibly because their caregivers use language in ways that support such development. Specifically, caregivers’ language is often fine-tuned to children’s current linguistic knowledge and vocabulary, providing an optimal level of complexity to support language learning. In their new research, Leung and colleagues add to the body of knowledge involving how caregivers foster children’s language acquisition.
The researchers asked individual parents to play a game with their child (age 2–2.5 years) in which they guided their child to select a target animal from a set. Without prompting, the parents provided more informative references for animals they thought their children did not know. For example, if a parent thought their child did not know the word “leopard,” they might use adjectives (“the spotted, yellow leopard”) or comparisons (“the one like a cat”). This indicates that parents adjust their references to account for their children’s language knowledge and vocabulary—not in a simplifying way but in a way that could increase the children’s vocabulary. Parents also appeared to learn about their children’s knowledge throughout the game and to adjust their references accordingly.
Infant and Adult Brains Are Coupled to the Dynamics of Natural Communication
Elise A. Piazza, Liat Hasenfratz, Uri Hasson, and Casey Lew-Williams (2020)
This research tracked real-time brain activation during infant–adult interactions, providing an innovative measure of social interaction at an early age. When communicating with infants, adults appear to be sensitive to subtle cues that can modify their brain responses and behaviors to improve alignment with, and maximize information transfer to, the infants.
Piazza and colleagues used functional near-infrared spectroscopy—a noninvasive measure of blood oxygenation resulting from neural activity that is minimally affected by movements and thus allows participants to freely interact and move—to measure the brain activation of infants (9–15 months old) and adults while they communicated and played with each other. An adult experimenter either engaged directly with an infant by playing with toys, singing nursery rhymes, and reading a story or performed those same tasks while turned away from the child and toward another adult in the room.
Results indicated that when the adult interacted with the child (but not with the other adult), the activations of many prefrontal cortex (PFC) channels and some parietal channels were intercorrelated, indicating neural coupling of the adult’s and child’s brains. Both infant and adult PFC activation preceded moments of mutual gaze and increased before the infant smiled, with the infant’s PFC response preceding the adult’s. Infant PFC activity also preceded an increase in the pitch variability of the adult’s speech, although no changes occurred in the adult’s PFC, indicating that the adult’s speech influenced the infant but probably did not influence neural coupling between the child and the adult.
Theory-of-Mind Development in Young Deaf Children With Early Hearing Provisions
Chi-Lin Yu, Christopher M. Stanzione, Henry M. Wellman, and Amy R. Lederberg (2020)
Language and communication are important for social and cognitive development. Although deaf and hard-of-hearing (DHH) children born to deaf parents can communicate with their caregivers using sign language, most DHH children are born to hearing parents who do not have experience with sign language. These children may have difficulty with early communication and experience developmental delays. For instance, the development of theory of mind—the understanding of others’ mental states—is usually delayed in DHH children born to hearing parents.
Yu and colleagues studied how providing DHH children with hearing devices early in life (before 2 years of age) might enrich their early communication experiences and benefit their language development, supporting the typical development of other capabilities—in particular, theory of mind. The researchers show that 3- to 6-year-old DHH children who began using cochlear implants or hearing aids earlier had more advanced language abilities, leading to better theory-of-mind growth, than children who started using hearing provisions later. These findings highlight the relationships among hearing, language, and theory of mind.
The Bilingual Advantage in Children’s Executive Functioning Is Not Related to Language Status: A Meta-Analytic Review
Cassandra J. Lowe, Isu Cho, Samantha F. Goldsmith, and J. Bruce Morton (2021)
Acommon idea is that bilingual children, who grow up speaking two languages fluently, perform better than monolingual children in diverse executive-functioning domains (e.g., attention, working memory, decision making). This meta-analysis calls that idea into question.
Lowe and colleagues synthesized data from studies that compared the performance of monolingual and bilingual participants between the ages of 3 and 17 years in executive-functioning domains (1,194 effect sizes). They found only a small effect of bilingualism on participants’ executive functioning, which was largely explained by factors such as publication bias. After accounting for these factors, bilingualism had no distinguishable effect. The results of this large meta-analysis thus suggest that bilingual and monolingual children tend to perform at the same level in executive-functioning tasks. Bilingualism does not appear to boost performance in executive functions that serve learning, thinking, reasoning, or problem solving.
APS regularly opens certain online articles for discussion on our website. Effective February 2021, you must be a logged-in APS member to post comments. By posting a comment, you agree to our Community Guidelines and the display of your profile information, including your name and affiliation. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations present in article comments are those of the writers and do not necessarily reflect the views of APS or the article’s author. For more information, please see our Community Guidelines .
Please login with your APS account to comment.
Elika Bergelson’s Quest Into Infants’ Language Development
Elika Bergelson, an associate developmental psychology professor at Harvard University, is known for her work on language acquisition, cognitive development, and word learning in infants. Her key research focuses on how infants learn language from the world around them.
Teaching: Ethical Research to Help Romania’s Abandoned Children
An early intervention experiment in Bucharest can introduce students to the importance of responsive caregiving during human development.
Silver Linings in the Demographic Revolution
Podcast: In her final column as APS President, Alison Gopnik makes the case for more effectively and creatively caring for vulnerable humans at either end of life.
Privacy Overview
Cookie | Duration | Description |
---|---|---|
__cf_bm | 30 minutes | This cookie, set by Cloudflare, is used to support Cloudflare Bot Management. |
Cookie | Duration | Description |
---|---|---|
AWSELBCORS | 5 minutes | This cookie is used by Elastic Load Balancing from Amazon Web Services to effectively balance load on the servers. |
Cookie | Duration | Description |
---|---|---|
at-rand | never | AddThis sets this cookie to track page visits, sources of traffic and share counts. |
CONSENT | 2 years | YouTube sets this cookie via embedded youtube-videos and registers anonymous statistical data. |
uvc | 1 year 27 days | Set by addthis.com to determine the usage of addthis.com service. |
_ga | 2 years | The _ga cookie, installed by Google Analytics, calculates visitor, session and campaign data and also keeps track of site usage for the site's analytics report. The cookie stores information anonymously and assigns a randomly generated number to recognize unique visitors. |
_gat_gtag_UA_3507334_1 | 1 minute | Set by Google to distinguish users. |
_gid | 1 day | Installed by Google Analytics, _gid cookie stores information on how visitors use a website, while also creating an analytics report of the website's performance. Some of the data that are collected include the number of visitors, their source, and the pages they visit anonymously. |
Cookie | Duration | Description |
---|---|---|
loc | 1 year 27 days | AddThis sets this geolocation cookie to help understand the location of users who share the information. |
VISITOR_INFO1_LIVE | 5 months 27 days | A cookie set by YouTube to measure bandwidth that determines whether the user gets the new or old player interface. |
YSC | session | YSC cookie is set by Youtube and is used to track the views of embedded videos on Youtube pages. |
yt-remote-connected-devices | never | YouTube sets this cookie to store the video preferences of the user using embedded YouTube video. |
yt-remote-device-id | never | YouTube sets this cookie to store the video preferences of the user using embedded YouTube video. |
yt.innertube::nextId | never | This cookie, set by YouTube, registers a unique ID to store data on what videos from YouTube the user has seen. |
yt.innertube::requests | never | This cookie, set by YouTube, registers a unique ID to store data on what videos from YouTube the user has seen. |
Disclaimer » Advertising
- HealthyChildren.org
- Previous Article
- Next Article
The Importance of Language-Learning Environments to Child Language Outcomes
POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The author has indicated she has no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.
FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The author has indicated she has no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.
- Split-Screen
- Article contents
- Figures & tables
- Supplementary Data
- Peer Review
- CME Quiz Close Quiz
- Open the PDF for in another window
- Get Permissions
- Cite Icon Cite
- Search Site
Heidi M. Feldman; The Importance of Language-Learning Environments to Child Language Outcomes. Pediatrics October 2019; 144 (4): e20192157. 10.1542/peds.2019-2157
Download citation file:
- Ris (Zotero)
- Reference Manager
A strong foundation in language skills is associated with positive, long-term academic, occupational, and social outcomes. Individual differences in the rate of language development appear early. Approximately 16% of children experience delays in initial phases of language learning; approximately half of those show persistent difficulties that may lead to clinical disorders. 1 Because of the high prevalence of language disorders and lifelong implications of early delays, prevention is of utmost importance. Primary prevention takes place before any problems are detected, preventing the condition from occurring. Secondary prevention takes place after early detection of a disorder, resulting in a mild rather than severe variant. Children learn language from their interactions with caregivers in their environment. An obvious direction for both primary and secondary prevention is improving language-learning environments.
In the study entitled “Parenting Behavior and Child Language: A Meta-analysis” by Madigan et al 2 in this issue of Pediatrics , the authors summarize evidence about “2 primary types of parenting” in relation to child language outcomes. Sensitive responsiveness refers to a parent’s ability to perceive, interpret, and respond quickly and appropriately to the children’s signals. The authors assume that sensitive responsiveness implies contingent responding to foster coordinated communicative exchanges. Warmth refers to caregiver physical affection or positive affect with the child. In the meta-analysis, it was found that sensitive responsiveness and warmth both contributed to child language outcomes. The effect size was greater for sensitivity than warmth and greater in studies of children from low or diverse socioeconomic status (SES) than from high SES.
The authors have performed an excellent service by conducting this meta-analysis and presenting it to a pediatric audience. So important are the features of the learning environment to language development that they have been referred to as “language nutrition.” 3 Pediatric clinicians routinely counsel families about food nutrition. We should address language nutrition with similar urgency. Of course, demonstrating the association between parenting qualities and child outcomes, as in the meta-analysis, does not yet imply which, if any, interventions would successfully change the learning environment or improve child outcomes. Data are accumulating that home-based interventions can raise the level of sensitivity and warmth, 4 although the effectiveness of less-intense interventions must be evaluated.
It is worth noting that sensitivity and warmth do not necessarily represent 2 distinct parenting types but rather 2 features of parenting. Definitions of parental sensitivity in other studies include warmth as a feature. 4 Sensitive responsivity can be disentangled from qualities specifically related to caregiver-child connections in verbal exchange. 5 Precision in the description of language-learning environments is difficult because a meta-analysis depends on the researchers’ characterization.
Sensitivity and warmth do not represent all the critical ingredients of healthy language nutrition. Three other features have been associated with language outcomes. First is the quantity of child-directed speech. Hart and Risley 6 made seminal observations that children of highly educated parents heard many more words than children of less-educated parents and then had better language skills at school entry. Findings that quantity of input is important, irrespective of SES, have been made by using all-day-long audio recordings of the child’s language environment in English- and Spanish-speaking children 7 and in children born term and preterm. 8 These studies corroborate the importance of quantity of child-directed speech and collectively form the foundation of public policy efforts to reduce the “30 million word gap” before a child’s entering school. 9 Second is the quality of the language input. 10 Quality includes diversity of the vocabulary and complexity of grammar. Third is the nature of the caregiver-child interactions, beyond responsivity and warmth. 5 Important qualitative features include degree of caregiver-child engagement with symbols, such as words or signs; frequency of routines and rituals, such as naming or book-reading; and the connectedness of exchange, reflected in topic maintenance and turn-taking.
The proportion of the various ingredients that comprise language nutrition likely varies as a function of the child’s stage of development. 4 Early on, especially in infancy, parental sensitivity and warmth (the focus of the meta-analysis) likely has substantial impact. Once children have begun speaking, cognitive and language features of the input, reflected in quantity and quality of linguistic input and qualitative features of verbal interactions, likely become increasingly impactful. 4 Although features of the environment may be modifiable, promoting stable change and sustainable results may be challenging. 11
Meta-analyses on the topic of language development are extremely helpful. However, now we also need well-designed treatment studies to inform us about the nature and intensity of interventions to improve language-learning environments and child outcomes. On the basis of the results of the meta-analysis, primary care clinicians should educate caregivers about the importance of their parenting to their children’s language development. If families demonstrate limited warmth, responsiveness, or other components of language nutrition, it is imperative to counsel them and refer to community-based programs to educate and support them in improving their children’s language-learning environment.
Opinions expressed in these commentaries are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the American Academy of Pediatrics or its Committees.
FUNDING: Support for this work was provided by a grant from the NIH RO1- HD069150. Funded by the National Institutes of Health (NIH).
COMPANION PAPER: A companion to this article can be found online at www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/peds.2018-3556 .
socioeconomic status
Competing Interests
Advertising Disclaimer »
Citing articles via
Email alerts.
Affiliations
- Editorial Board
- Editorial Policies
- Journal Blogs
- Pediatrics On Call
- Online ISSN 1098-4275
- Print ISSN 0031-4005
- Pediatrics Open Science
- Hospital Pediatrics
- Pediatrics in Review
- AAP Grand Rounds
- Latest News
- Pediatric Care Online
- Red Book Online
- Pediatric Patient Education
- AAP Toolkits
- AAP Pediatric Coding Newsletter
First 1,000 Days Knowledge Center
Institutions/librarians, group practices, licensing/permissions, integrations, advertising.
- Privacy Statement | Accessibility Statement | Terms of Use | Support Center | Contact Us
- © Copyright American Academy of Pediatrics
This Feature Is Available To Subscribers Only
Sign In or Create an Account
Login | Register
- Editorial Team
- Editorial Policies
- Author Guidelines
- Reviewer Guidelines
- Statistics and Methods
- Research Integrity
- Full Policies
Language Development Research: An Open-Science Journal
Read author guidelines (PDF) before submission, and ensure your article includes a standalone Data, Code and Materials Availability Statement.
Science is for everyone. We set up Language Development Research (ISSN 2771-7976) because we don't believe in locking articles behind paywalls, in charging taxpayers and universities to publish research they've already funded, or in privileging papers that are "exciting" over those committed to scientific rigour. We believe that open science is better science. We uphold the highest standards of research integrity . We insist on open data and materials , and commit to publishing every article that is judged by our peer review process to meet our criteria for methodological and theoretical rigour .
We invite submissions of empirical and theoretical investigations of children's language development : typical and atypical, mono-, bi- and multi-lingual, spoken, signed, or written. We are also interested in the exploration of any topic or population relevant to language development, broadly construed (e.g., second language learning, artificial language learning, adult psycholinguistics, computational modeling).
Fiercely independent, we are answerable to no one except the scientific community and our 30-member strong Editorial Board of respected researchers . Please browse our articles (below on this page), learn more about the journal and its editorial policies , and, when you're ready, submit your article .
Featured Articles
David Pagmar, Kirsten Abbot-Smith, Danielle Matthews
Predictors of children's conversational contingency
Evelina Leivada, Elliot Murphy
A demonstration of the uncomputability of parametric models of language acquisition and a biologically plausible alternative
alejandrina cristia, Marisa Casillas
Non-word repetition in children learning Yélî Dnye
Linda Kelly, Elizabeth Nixon, Jean Quigley
It’s Your Turn: The Dynamics of Conversational Turn-Taking in Father-Child and Mother-Child Interaction
Khazar Khorrami, Okko Räsänen
Can phones, syllables, and words emerge as side-products of cross-situational audiovisual learning? - A computational investigation
Karla K McGregor, Ronald Pomper, Nichole Eden, Timothy Arbisi-Kelm, Nancy Ohlmann, Shivani Gajre, Erin Smolak
Children’s language abilities predict success in remote communication contexts
Disa Witkowska, Laura Lucas, Maria Jelen, Hannah Kin, Courtenay Norbury
Development of complex syntax in the narratives of children with English as an Additional Language and their monolingual peers
Giulia Bovolenta, Emma Marsden
Expectation violation enhances the development of new abstract syntactic representations: evidence from an artificial language learning study
Nicola Dawson, Yaling Hsiao, Alvin Wei Ming Tan, Nilanjana Banerji, Kate Nation
Features of lexical richness in children's books: Comparisons with child-directed speech
Ben Ambridge
Language Development Research Editorial: Why do we need another journal?
Mitja Nikolaus, Eliot Maes, Jeremy Auguste, Laurent Prévot, Abdellah Fourtassi
Large-scale study of speech acts' development in early childhood
Audun Rosslund, Julien Mayor, Gabriella Óturai, Natalia Kartushina
Parents’ hyper-pitch and low vowel category variability in infant-directed speech are associated with 18-month-old toddlers’ expressive vocabulary
Samuel David Jones, Madeline Dooley, Ben Ambridge
Passive sentence reversal errors in autism: Replicating Ambridge, Bidgood, and Thomas (2020)
Victoria Knowland, Mohreet Rauni, Gareth Gaskell, Sarah Walker, Elaine van Rijn, Courtenay Norbury, Lisa-Marie Henderson
Sleep behaviour in children with developmental language disorder
Benjamin Edward deMayo, Danielle Kellier, Mika Braginsky, Christina Bergmann, Cielke Hendriks, Caroline Frances Rowland, Michael C Frank, Virginia Marchman
Web-CDI: A system for online administration of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories
Maxime Alexandra Tulling, Ailís Cournane
Wishes before ifs: mapping “fake” past tense to counterfactuality in wishes and conditionals
Natalia Kartushina, Nivedita Mani, Aslı Aktan-Erciyes, Khadeejah Alaslani, Naomi J. Aldrich, Alaa Almohammadi, Haifa Alroqi, Lucy M. Anderson, Elena Andonova, Suzanne Aussems, Mireille Babineau, Mihaela Barokova, Christina Bergmann, Cara Cashon, Stephanie Custode, Alex de Carvalho, Nevena Dimitrova, Agnieszka Dynak, Rola Farah, Christopher Fennell, Anne-Caroline Fiévet, Michael C Frank, Margarita Gavrilova, Hila Gendler-Shalev, Shannon P. Gibson, Katherine Golway, Nayeli Gonzalez-Gomez, Ewa Haman, Erin Hannon, Naomi Havron, Jessica Hay, Cielke Hendriks, Tzipi Horowitz-kraus, Marina Kalashnikova, Junko Kanero, Christina Keller, Grzegorz Krajewski, Catherine Laing, Rebecca A. Lundwall, Magdalena Łuniewska, Karolina Mieszkowska, Luis Muñoz, Karli Nave, Nonah Olesen, Lynn Perry, Caroline Frances Rowland, Daniela Santos Oliveira, Jeanne Shinskey, Aleksander Veraksa, Kolbie Vincent, Michal Zivan, Julien Mayor
COVID-19 first lockdown as a window into language acquisition: associations between caregiver-child activities and vocabulary gains
Iris Broedelet, Paul Boersma, Judith Rispens
Distributional learning of novel visual object categories in children with and without developmental language disorder
Martin Fortier, Danielle Kellier, María Fernández-Flecha, Michael C Frank
Ad-hoc pragmatic implicatures among Shipibo-Konibo children in the Peruvian Amazon
Matt Hilton, Katherine E. Twomey, Gert Westermann
Face time: Effects of shyness and attention to faces on early word learning
Robert Fromont, Lynn Clark, Joshua Wilson Black, Margaret Blackwood
Maximizing accuracy of forced alignment for spontaneous child speech
Anika van der Klis, Rianne van Lieburg, Lisa Lai-Shen Cheng, Clara Cecilia Levelt
Pauses matter: Rule-learning in children
Joshua Hartshorne, Yujing Huang, Lauren Skorb
Some puzzling findings regarding the acquisition of verbs
Ben Ambridge, Stewart McCauley, Colin Bannard, Michelle Davis, Thea Cameron-Faulkner, Alison Gummery, Anna Theakston
Uninversion error in English-speaking children’s wh-questions: Blame it on the bigrams?
Janet Yougi Bang, George Kachergis, Adriana Weisleder, Virginia Marchman
An automated classifier for periods of sleep and target-child-directed speech from LENA recordings
Lena V. Kremin, Amel Jardak, Casey Lew-Williams, Krista Byers-Heinlein
Bilingual children’s comprehension of code-switching at an uninformative adjective
Ana Maria Gonzalez-Barrero, Rodrigo Dal Ben, Hilary Killam, Krista Byers-Heinlein
Word learning in 14-month-old monolinguals and bilinguals: Challenges and methodological opportunities
Elspeth Wilson, Kate Cain, Catherine Davies, Jenny Gibson, Holly Joseph, Ludovica Serratrice, Margreet Vogelzang
Children’s development of conversational and reading inference skills: a call for a collaborative approach
Elizabeth Swanson, Michael C Frank, Judith Degen
Syntactic adaptation and word learning in children and adults
Enkeleida Kapia, Shanley Allen, Doruntinë Zogaj
Early vocabulary and grammar development in Albanian-speaking children: a MB-CDI adaptation study
Emma Thornton, Praveetha Patalay, Danielle Matthews, Colin Bannard
Investigating how vocabulary relates to different dimensions of family socio-economic circumstance across developmental and historical time
Sivan Bar-Or, Naomi Havron
Is the effect of gross motor development on vocabulary size mediated by language-promoting interactions?
Houjun Liu, Brian MacWhinney
Morphosyntactic Analysis for CHILDES
- Tools and Resources
- Customer Services
- Affective Science
- Biological Foundations of Psychology
- Clinical Psychology: Disorders and Therapies
- Cognitive Psychology/Neuroscience
- Developmental Psychology
- Educational/School Psychology
- Forensic Psychology
- Health Psychology
- History and Systems of Psychology
- Individual Differences
- Methods and Approaches in Psychology
- Neuropsychology
- Organizational and Institutional Psychology
- Personality
- Psychology and Other Disciplines
- Social Psychology
- Sports Psychology
- Share Facebook LinkedIn Twitter
Article contents
Language development.
- Carolyn Quam Carolyn Quam Portland State University
- , and Teresa Roberts Teresa Roberts Portland State University
- https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.57
- Published online: 23 August 2023
Language is a complex human capacity. The speed with which young children learn it is a remarkable feat. Across diverse cultures and family structures, children learn thousands of languages. Despite tremendous variation across languages, commonalities hold in structure and learning mechanisms. Children undergo perceptual and expressive development. They learn to organize and process language structure at the levels of semantics, phonology, morphology, syntax, and pragmatics. Rates of learning of linguistic patterns are shaped by frequency, complexity, concreteness of form-to-meaning mappings, and the number of exceptions to a rule. In languages with written forms, children apply language knowledge to reading and writing.
- child development
- grammatical development
- language acquisition
- language development
- perceptual learning
- social communication
- word learning
You do not currently have access to this article
Please login to access the full content.
Access to the full content requires a subscription
Printed from Oxford Research Encyclopedias, Psychology. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a single article for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
date: 29 September 2024
- Cookie Policy
- Privacy Policy
- Legal Notice
- Accessibility
- [185.194.105.172]
- 185.194.105.172
Character limit 500 /500
- A-Z Publications
Annual Review of Developmental Psychology
Volume 2, 2020, review article, language development in context.
- Meredith L. Rowe 1 , and Adriana Weisleder 2
- View Affiliations Hide Affiliations Affiliations: 1 Graduate School of Education, Harvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02138, USA; email: [email protected] 2 Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois 60610, USA
- Vol. 2:201-223 (Volume publication date December 2020) https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-042220-121816
- First published as a Review in Advance on September 15, 2020
- Copyright © 2020 by Annual Reviews. All rights reserved
Young children learn to communicate in the language(s) of their communities, yet the individual trajectories of language development and the particular language varieties and modes of communication children acquire vary depending on the contexts in which they live. This review describes how context shapes language development. Building on the bioecological model of development, we conceptualize context as a set of nested systems surrounding the child, from the national policies and cultural norms that shape the broader environment to the particular communicative interactions in which children experience language being used. In addition, we describe how children's developing sensory-motor, perceptual, and social-cognitive capacities respond to and are tuned by the surrounding environment. Closer integration of research on the mechanisms of language learning with investigation of the contexts in which this learning takes place will provide critical insights into the process of language development.
Article metrics loading...
Full text loading...
Literature Cited
- Adamson LB , Kaiser AP , Tamis-LaMonda CS , Owen MT , Dimitrova N 2020 . The developmental landscape of early parent-focused language intervention. Early Child. Res. Q. 50 : 59– 67 [Google Scholar]
- Akhtar N. 2005 . The robustness of learning through overhearing. Dev. Sci. 8 : 2 199– 209 [Google Scholar]
- Aukrust VG , Snow CE. 1998 . Narratives and explanations during mealtime conversations in Norway and the U.S. Lang. Soc. 27 : 2 221– 46 [Google Scholar]
- Benasich AA , Tallal P. 2002 . Infant discrimination of rapid auditory cues predicts later language impairment. Behav. Brain Res. 136 : 1 31– 49 [Google Scholar]
- Benitez VL , Smith LB 2012 . Predictable locations aid early object name learning. Cognition 125 : 3 339– 52 [Google Scholar]
- Bergelson E , Casillas M , Soderstrom M , Seidl A , Warlaumont AS , Amatuni A 2019 . What do North American babies hear? A large-scale cross-corpus analysis. Dev. Sci. 22 : 1 e12724 [Google Scholar]
- Biel CH , Buzhardt J , Brown JA , Romano MK , Lorio CM et al. 2020 . Language interventions taught to caregivers in homes and classrooms: a review of intervention and implementation fidelity. Early Child. Res. Q. 50 : 140– 56 [Google Scholar]
- Bloom P. 2002 . How Children Learn the Meanings of Words Cambridge, MA: MIT Press [Google Scholar]
- Bohn M , Frank MC. 2019 . The pervasive role of pragmatics in early language. Annu. Rev. Dev. Psychol. 1 : 223– 49 [Google Scholar]
- Bridges K , Hoff E. 2014 . Older sibling influences on the language environment and language development of toddlers in bilingual homes. App. Psycholing. 35 : 2 225– 41 [Google Scholar]
- Britto PR , Lye SJ , Proulx K , Yousafzai AK , Matthews SG et al. 2017 . Nurturing care: promoting early childhood development. Lancet 389 : 10064 91– 102 [Google Scholar]
- Bronfenbrenner U , Ceci SJ. 1994 . Nature-nurture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: a bioecological model. Psychol. Rev. 101 : 568– 86 [Google Scholar]
- Brown P. 2011 . The cultural organization of attention. The Handbook of Language Socialization A Duranti, E Ochs, BB Schieffelin 29– 55 Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons [Google Scholar]
- Bruner JS. 1985 . The role of interaction formats in language acquisition. Language and Social Situations JP Forgas 31– 46 New York: Springer [Google Scholar]
- Burchinal M , Nelson L , Carlson M , Brooks-Gunn J 2008 . Neighborhood characteristics and child care type and quality. Early Educ. Dev. 19 : 5 702– 25 [Google Scholar]
- Bustamante AS , Hassinger-Das B , Hirsh-Pasek K , Golinkoff RM 2019 . Learning landscapes: where the science of learning meets architectural design. Child Dev. Persp. 13 : 1 34– 40 [Google Scholar]
- Cartmill EA , Armstrong BF , Gleitman LR , Goldin-Meadow S , Medina TN , Trueswell JC 2013 . Quality of early parent input predicts child vocabulary 3 years later. PNAS 110 : 28 11278– 83 [Google Scholar]
- Casillas M , Brown P , Levinson SC 2020 . Early language experience in a Tseltal Mayan village. Child Dev 91 : 1819 – 35 [Google Scholar]
- Cates CB , Weisleder A , Berkule Johnson S , Seery AM , Canfield CF et al. 2018 . Enhancing parent talk, reading, and play in primary care: sustained impacts of the video interaction project. J. Pediatr. 199 : 49– 56 [Google Scholar]
- Chouinard MM. 2007 . Children's questions: a mechanism for cognitive development: III. Diary study of children's questions. Children's Questions: A Mechanism for Cognitive Development MM Chouinard, PL Harris, MP Maratsos 45– 57 Boston, MA: Blackwell [Google Scholar]
- Clarke-Stewart KA. 1978 . And Daddy makes three: the father's impact on mother and young child. Child Dev 49 : 2 466– 78 [Google Scholar]
- Corriveau KH , Kinzler KD , Harris PL 2013 . Accuracy trumps accent in children's endorsement of object labels. Dev. Psychol. 49 : 3 470– 79 [Google Scholar]
- Cristia A , Dupoux E , Gurven M , Stieglitz J 2019 . Child-directed speech is infrequent in a forager-farmer population: a time allocation study. Child Dev 90 : 3 759– 73 [Google Scholar]
- Deckner DF , Adamson LB , Bakeman R 2006 . Child and maternal contributions to shared reading: effects on language and literacy development. J. App. Dev. Psychol. 27 : 1 31– 41 [Google Scholar]
- de León L. 2011 . Language socialization and multiparty participation frameworks. The Handbook of Language Socialization A Duranti, E Ochs, BB Schieffelin 81– 111 Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons [Google Scholar]
- DeLoache JS , Chiong C , Sherman K , Islam N , Vanderborght M et al. 2010 . Do babies learn from baby media. ? Psychol. Sci. 21 : 11 1570– 74 [Google Scholar]
- De Marco A , Vernon-Feagans L 2013 . Rural neighborhood context, child care quality, and relationship to early language development. Early Educ. Dev. 24 : 6 792– 812 [Google Scholar]
- Dunn J , Shatz M. 1989 . Becoming a conversationalist despite (or because of) having an older sibling. Child Dev 60 : 2 399– 410 [Google Scholar]
- Elmlinger SL , Schwade JA , Goldstein MH 2019 . The ecology of prelinguistic vocal learning: Parents simplify the structure of their speech in response to babbling. J. Child Lang. 16 : 998– 1011 [Google Scholar]
- Fausey CM , Jayaraman S , Smith LB 2016 . From faces to hands: changing visual input in the first two years. Cognition 152 : 101– 7 [Google Scholar]
- Fenson L , Dale PS , Reznick JS , Bates E , Thal DJ , Pethick SJ 1994 . Variability in Early Communicative Development Chicago, IL: Univ. Chicago Press [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson B , Graf E , Waxman SR 2014 . Infants use known verbs to learn novel nouns: evidence from 15- and 19-month-olds. Cognition 131 : 1 139– 46 [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson B , Graf E , Waxman SR 2018 . When veps cry: Two-year-olds efficiently learn novel words from linguistic contexts alone. Lang. Learn. Dev. 14 : 1 1– 12 [Google Scholar]
- Ferjan-Ramírez N , Lytle SR , Fish M , Kuhl PK 2019 . Parent coaching at 6 and 10 months improves language outcomes at 14 months: a randomized controlled trial. Dev. Sci. 22 : 3 e12762 [Google Scholar]
- Fernald A , Perfors A , Marchman VA 2006 . Picking up speed in understanding: speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. Dev. Psychol. 42 : 1 98– 116 [Google Scholar]
- Floccia C , Sambrook TD , Luche CD , Kwok R , Goslin J et al. 2018 . Vocabulary of 2‐Year‐Olds Learning English and an Additional Language: Norms and Effects of Linguistic Distance Boston, MA: Wiley [Google Scholar]
- Flores N , Rosa J. 2015 . Undoing appropriateness: raciolinguistic ideologies and language diversity in education. Harv. Educ. Rev. 85 : 149– 71 [Google Scholar]
- Flores N , Rosa J. 2019 . Bringing race into second language acquisition. Mod. Lang. J. 103 : 145– 51 [Google Scholar]
- Frank MC , Braginsky M , Marchman VA , Yurovsky D 2021 . Variability and Consistency in Early Language Learning: The Wordbank Project Cambridge, MA: MIT Press In press [Google Scholar]
- Frazier BN , Gelman SA , Wellman HM 2009 . Preschoolers’ search for explanatory information within adult–child conversation. Child Dev 80 : 6 1592– 611 [Google Scholar]
- Genesee F. 2006 . Bilingual first language acquisition in perspective. Childhood Bilingualism E Hoff, P McCardle 45– 67 Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters [Google Scholar]
- Gleason JB. 1975 . Fathers and other strangers: men's speech to young children. Developmental Psycholinguistics: Theory and Applications DP Dato 289– 97 Washington, DC: Georgetown Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
- Goldenberg C , Wagner K. 2015 . Bilingual education: reviving an American tradition. Am. Educ. 39 : 3 29– 44 [Google Scholar]
- Goldstein MH , Schwade JA. 2008 . Social feedback to infants’ babbling facilitates rapid phonological learning. Psych. Sci. 19 : 5 515– 23 [Google Scholar]
- Golinkoff RM , Ames GJ. 1979 . A comparison of fathers' and mothers' speech with their young children. Child Dev 50 : 1 28– 32 [Google Scholar]
- Greenfield PM. 2009 . Linking social change and developmental change: shifting pathways of human development. Dev. Psychol. 45 : 2 401– 18 [Google Scholar]
- Hart B , Risley TR. 1995 . Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children Baltimore, MD: BROOKES [Google Scholar]
- Havron N , Ramus F , Heude B , Forhan A , Cristia A et al. 2019 . The effect of older siblings on language development as a function of age difference and sex. Psychol. Sci. 30 : 9 1333– 43 [Google Scholar]
- Heath SB. 1983 . Ways with Words: Language, Life and Work in Communities and Classrooms Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
- Heidlage JK , Cunningham JE , Kaiser AP , Trivette CM , Barton EE et al. 2020 . The effects of parent-implemented language interventions on child linguistic outcomes: a meta-analysis. Comm. Sci. Dis. 50 : 6– 23 [Google Scholar]
- Hirsh-Pasek K , Adamson LB , Bakeman R , Owen MT , Golinkoff RM et al. 2015 . The contribution of early communication quality to low-income children's language success. Psychol. Sci. 26 : 7 1071– 83 [Google Scholar]
- Hoff E. 2003 . The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Dev 74 : 5 1368– 78 [Google Scholar]
- Hoff E. 2006 . How social contexts support and shape language development. Dev. Rev. 26 : 55– 88 [Google Scholar]
- Hoff E. 2013 . Interpreting the early language trajectories of children from low-SES and language minority homes: implications for closing achievement gaps. Dev. Psychol. 49 : 1 4– 14 [Google Scholar]
- Hoff-Ginsberg E. 1991 . Mother-child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Dev 62 : 4 782– 96 [Google Scholar]
- Hoff-Ginsberg E. 1998 . The relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to children's language experience and language development. App. Psycholing. 19 : 4 603– 29 [Google Scholar]
- Hoff-Ginsberg E , Krueger WM. 1991 . Older siblings as conversational partners. Merrill-Palmer Q 37 : 3 465– 81 [Google Scholar]
- Hurtado N , Marchman VA , Fernald A 2008 . Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Dev. Sci. 11 : 6 F31– 39 [Google Scholar]
- Huttenlocher J , Haight W , Bryk A , Seltzer M , Lyons T 1991 . Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Dev. Psychol. 27 : 2 236– 48 [Google Scholar]
- Huttenlocher J , Waterfall HR , Vasilyeva M , Vevea JL , Hedges LV 2010 . Sources of variability in children's language growth. Cog. Psychol. 61 : 4 343– 65 [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins JR , Stein ML , Wysocki K 1984 . Learning vocabulary through reading. Am. Educ. Res. J. 21 : 4 767– 87 [Google Scholar]
- Jones MN , Mewhort DJ. 2007 . Representing word meaning and order information in a composite holographic lexicon. Psychol. Rev. 114 : 1 1– 37 [Google Scholar]
- Karasik LB , Tamis‐LeMonda CS , Adolph KE 2011 . Transition from crawling to walking and infants’ actions with objects and people. Child Dev 82 : 4 1199– 209 [Google Scholar]
- Karasik LB , Tamis-LeMonda CS , Adolph KE 2014 . Crawling and walking infants elicit different verbal responses from mothers. Dev. Sci. 17 : 3 388– 95 [Google Scholar]
- Keller H. 2003 . Socialization for competence: cultural models of infancy. Hum. Dev. 46 : 5 288– 311 [Google Scholar]
- Kinzler KD , Corriveau KH , Harris PL 2011 . Children's selective trust in native-accented speakers. Dev. Sci. 14 : 1 106– 11 [Google Scholar]
- Koenig MA , Harris PL. 2005 . Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Dev 76 : 6 1261– 77 [Google Scholar]
- Kruper JC , Uzgiris I. 1987 . Fathers' and mothers' speech to young infants. J. Psycholing. Res. 6 : 597– 614 [Google Scholar]
- Kuhl PK. 2007 . Is speech learning ‘gated’ by the social brain. ? Dev. Sci. 10 : 1 110– 20 [Google Scholar]
- Kuhl PK , Tsao F-M , Lium H-M 2003 . Foreign-language experience in infancy: effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. PNAS 100 : 15 9096– 101 [Google Scholar]
- Lany J , Giglio M , Oswald M 2018 . Infants’ lexical processing efficiency is related to vocabulary size by one year of age. Infancy 23 : 3 342– 66 [Google Scholar]
- Lareau A. 2011 . Unequal Childhoods: Class, Race, and Family Life Berkeley, CA: Univ. California Press. , 2nd ed.. [Google Scholar]
- Leaper C , Anderson KJ , Sanders P 1998 . Moderators of gender effects on parents' talk to their children: a meta-analysis. Dev. Psychol. 34 : 3– 27 [Google Scholar]
- Leech K , Rowe ML. 2020 . An intervention to increase conversational turns between parents and young children. J. Child Lang. 2020 : 1– 14 [Google Scholar]
- Leech K , Wei R , Harring JR , Rowe ML 2018 . A brief parent-focused intervention to improve preschoolers' conversational skills and school readiness. Dev. Psychol. 54 : 1 15– 28 [Google Scholar]
- Leung CYY , Hernandez MW , Suskind DL 2020 . Enriching home language environment among families from low-SES backgrounds: a randomized controlled trial of a home visiting curriculum. Early Child. Res. Q. 50 : 24– 35 [Google Scholar]
- Leung CYY , Suskind DL. 2020 . What parents know matters: Parental knowledge at birth predicts caregiving behaviors at 9 months. J. Pediatr. 221 : 72– 80 [Google Scholar]
- LeVine RA , LeVine S , Schnell-Anzola B , Rowe ML , Dexter E 2012 . Literacy and Mothering: How Women's Schooling Changes the Lives of the World's Children Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
- Lidz J , Gagliardi A. 2015 . How nature meets nurture: universal grammar and statistical learning. Annu. Rev. Ling. 1 : 333– 53 [Google Scholar]
- Lieven EVM. 1994 . Crosslinguistic and crosscultural aspects of language addressed to children. Input and Interaction in Language Acquisition C Gallaway, BJ Richards 56– 73 Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
- Lloyd JE , Li L , Hertzman C 2010 . Early experiences matter: lasting effect of concentrated disadvantage on children's language and cognitive outcomes. Health Place 16 : 371– 80 [Google Scholar]
- Macintyre S , Ellaway A , Cummins S 2002 . Place effects on health: How can we conceptualise, operationalise and measure them. ? Soc. Sci. Med. 55 : 1 125– 39 [Google Scholar]
- Mahr T , Edwards J. 2018 . Using language input and lexical processing to predict vocabulary size. Dev. Sci. 21 : e12685 [Google Scholar]
- Malin JL , Cabrera NJ , Rowe ML 2014 . Low-income minority mothers' and fathers' reading and children's interest: longitudinal contributions to children's receptive vocabulary skills. Early Child. Res. Q. 29 : 4 425– 32 [Google Scholar]
- McGillion M , Pine JM , Herbert JS , Matthews D 2017 . A randomised controlled trial to test the effect of promoting caregiver contingent talk on language development in infants from diverse socioeconomic status backgrounds. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 58 : 10 1122– 31 [Google Scholar]
- Mendelsohn AL , Valdez PT , Flynn V , Foley GM , Berkule SB et al. 2007 . Use of videotaped interactions during pediatric well-child care: impact at 33 months on parenting and on child development. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 28 : 3 206– 12 [Google Scholar]
- Minh A , Muhajarine N , Janus M , Brownell M , Guhn M 2017 . A review of neighborhood effects and early child development: How, where, and for whom, do neighborhoods matter. ? Health Place 46 : 155– 74 [Google Scholar]
- Montag JL. 2019 . Differences in sentence complexity in the text of children's picture books and child-directed speech. First Lang 39 : 5 527– 46 [Google Scholar]
- Montag JL , Jones MN , Smith LB 2015 . The words children hear: picture books and the statistics for language learning. Psychol. Sci. 26 : 9 1489– 96 [Google Scholar]
- Montag JL , Jones MN , Smith LB 2018 . Quantity and diversity: simulating early word learning environments. Cogn. Sci. 42 : 375– 412 [Google Scholar]
- Morelli G , Bard K , Chaudhary N , Gottlieb A , Keller H et al. 2018 . Bringing the real world into developmental science: a commentary on Weber, Fernald, and Diop (2017). Child Dev 89 : 6 e594– 603 [Google Scholar]
- Nelson K. 1985 . Making Sense: The Acquisition of Shared Meaning New York: Academic [Google Scholar]
- Neuman SB , Celano D. 2001 . Access to print in low-income and middle-income communities: an ecological study of four neighborhoods. Reading Res. Q. 36 : 1 8– 26 [Google Scholar]
- Neville HJ , Stevens C , Pakulak E , Bell TA , Fanning J et al. 2013 . Family-based training program improves brain function, cognition, and behavior in lower socioeconomic status preschoolers. PNAS 110 : 29 12138– 43 [Google Scholar]
- Newman RS , Rowe ML , Bernstein Ratner N 2016 . Input and uptake at 7 months predicts toddler vocabulary: the role of child-directed speech and infant processing skills in language development. J. Child Lang. 43 : 5 1158– 73 [Google Scholar]
- Ochs E , Schieffelin B. 1984 . Language acquisition and socialization. Culture Theory: Essays on Mind, Self, and Emotion RA Shweder, RA LeVine 276– 320 Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
- Ochs E , Schieffelin BB 2011 . The theory of language socialization. Handbook of Language Socialization E Duranti, E Ochs, BB Schieffelin 1– 21 Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell [Google Scholar]
- Pan BA , Rowe ML , Singer JD , Snow CE 2005 . Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Dev 76 : 4 763– 82 [Google Scholar]
- Pancsofar N , Vernon-Feagans L Fam. Life Proj. Investig 2010 . Fathers’ early contributions to children's language development in families from low-income rural communities. Early Child. Res. Q 25 : 4 450– 63 [Google Scholar]
- Pasquini ES , Corriveau KH , Koenig M , Harris PL 2007 . Preschoolers monitor the relative accuracy of informants. Dev. Psychol. 43 : 5 1216– 26 [Google Scholar]
- Perry LK , Prince EB , Valtierra AM , Rivero-Fernandez C , Ullery MA et al. 2018 . A year in words: the dynamics and consequences of language experiences in an intervention classroom. PLOS ONE 13 : 7 e0199893 [Google Scholar]
- Perszyk DR , Waxman SR. 2018 . Linking language and cognition in infancy. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 69 : 231– 50 [Google Scholar]
- Redington M , Chater N , Finch S 1998 . Distributional information: a powerful cue for acquiring syntactic categories. Cog. Sci. 22 : 4 425– 69 [Google Scholar]
- Renzi DT , Romberg AR , Bolger DJ , Newman RS 2017 . Two minds are better than one: cooperative communication as a new framework for understanding infant language learning. Transl. Issues Psychol. Sci. 3 : 1 19– 33 [Google Scholar]
- Reuter T , Emberson L , Romberg A , Lew-Williams C 2018 . Individual differences in nonverbal prediction and vocabulary size in infancy. Cognition 176 : 215– 19 [Google Scholar]
- Rice ML , Huston AC , Truglio R , Wright JC 1990 . Words from “Sesame Street”: learning vocabulary while viewing. Dev. Psychol. 26 : 3 421– 28 [Google Scholar]
- Richman AL , Miller PM , LeVine RA 1992 . Cultural and educational variations in maternal responsiveness. Dev. Psychol. 28 : 4 614– 21 [Google Scholar]
- Rickford JR , Duncan GJ , Gennetian LA , Gou RY , Greene R et al. 2015 . Neighborhood effects on use of African-American Vernacular English. PNAS 112 : 38 11817– 22 [Google Scholar]
- Rogoff B. 2003 . The Cultural Nature of Development Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
- Rojas R , Iglesias A , Bunta F , Goldstein B , Goldenberg C , Reese L 2016 . Interlocutor differential effects on the expressive language skills of Spanish-speaking English learners. Int. J. Speech-Lang. Path. 18 : 166– 77 [Google Scholar]
- Romeo RR , Leonard JA , Robinson ST , West MR , Mackey AP et al. 2018 . Beyond the 30-million-word gap: Children's conversational exposure is associated with language-related brain function. Psychol. Sci. 29 : 5 700– 10 [Google Scholar]
- Roseberry S , Hirsh-Pasek K , Golinkoff RM 2014 . Skype me! Socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language. Child Dev 85 : 3 956– 70 [Google Scholar]
- Rosemberg CR , Alam F , Audisio CP , Ramirez ML , Garber L , Migdalek MJ 2020 . Nouns and verbs in the linguistic environment of Argentinian toddlers: socioeconomic and context-related differences. First Lang 40 : 2 192– 217 [Google Scholar]
- Rowe ML. 2008 . Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. J. Child Lang. 35 : 185– 205 [Google Scholar]
- Rowe ML. 2012 . A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Dev 83 : 5 1762– 74 [Google Scholar]
- Rowe ML , Coker D , Pan BA 2004 . A comparison of fathers’ and mothers’ talk to toddlers in low‐income families. Soc. Dev. 13 : 2 278– 91 [Google Scholar]
- Rowe ML , Leech KA. 2019 . A parent intervention with a growth mindset approach improves children's early gesture and vocabulary development. Dev. Sci. 22 : e12792 [Google Scholar]
- Rowe ML , Leech KA , Cabrera N 2017 . Going beyond input quantity: Wh -questions matter for toddlers' language and cognitive development. Cog. Sci. 41 : 1 162– 79 [Google Scholar]
- Rowe ML , Snow CE. 2020 . Analyzing input quality along three dimensions: interactive, linguistic, and conceptual. J. Child Lang. 47 : 1 5– 21 [Google Scholar]
- Roy BC , Frank MC , DeCamp P , Miller M , Roy D 2015 . Predicting the birth of a spoken word. PNAS 112 : 41 12663– 68 [Google Scholar]
- Saffran JR. 2002 . Constraints on statistical language learning. J. Mem. Lang. 47 : 1 172– 96 [Google Scholar]
- Saffran JR , Aslin RN , Newport EL 1996 . Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science 274 : 5294 1926– 28 [Google Scholar]
- Saffran JR , Kirkham NZ. 2018 . Infant statistical learning. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 69 : 181– 203 [Google Scholar]
- Sayer LC , Gauthier AH , Furstenberg FF 2004 . Educational differences in parents? Time with children: cross-national variations. J. Marriage Fam. 66 : 5 1152– 69 [Google Scholar]
- Shafto P , Eaves B , Navarro DJ , Perfors A 2012 . Epistemic trust: modeling children's reasoning about others’ knowledge and intent. Dev. Sci. 15 : 3 436– 47 [Google Scholar]
- Shneidman LA , Arroyo ME , Levine SC , Goldin-Meadow S 2013 . What counts as effective input for word learning. ? J. Child Lang. 40 : 3 672– 86 [Google Scholar]
- Shneidman LA , Buresh JS , Shimpi PM , Knight-Schwarz J , Woodward AL 2009 . Social experience, social attention and word learning in an overhearing paradigm. Lang. Learn. Dev. 5 : 266– 81 [Google Scholar]
- Shneidman LA , Goldin-Meadow S. 2012 . Language input and acquisition in a Mayan village: How important is directed speech. ? Dev. Sci. 15 : 5 659– 73 [Google Scholar]
- Shneidman LA , Woodward AL. 2016 . Are child-directed interactions the cradle of social learning. ? Psychol. Bull. 142 : 1 1– 17 [Google Scholar]
- Singh L , Steven Reznick J , Xuehua L 2012 . Infant word segmentation and childhood vocabulary development: a longitudinal analysis. Dev. Sci. 15 : 4 482– 95 [Google Scholar]
- Smith L , Yu C. 2008 . Infants rapidly learn word-referent mappings via cross-situational statistics. Cognition 106 : 3 1558– 68 [Google Scholar]
- Smith LB , Jayaraman S , Clerkin E , Yu C 2018 . The developing infant creates a curriculum for statistical learning. Trends Cog. Sci. 22 : 4 325– 36 [Google Scholar]
- Snow CE. 1972 . Mothers' speech to children learning language. Child Dev 43 : 549– 65 [Google Scholar]
- Soderstrom M , Wittebolle K. 2013 . When do caregivers talk? The influences of activity and time of day on caregiver speech and child vocalizations in two childcare environments. PLOS ONE 8 : 11 e80646 [Google Scholar]
- Sorenson Duncan T , Paradis J 2020 . Home language environment and children's second language acquisition: the special status of input from older siblings. J. Child Lang. 2020 : 1– 24 [Google Scholar]
- Song L , Tamis-LeMonda CS , Yoshikawa H , Kahana-Kalman R , Wu I 2012 . Language experiences and vocabulary development in Dominican and Mexican infants across the first 2 years. Dev. Psychol. 48 : 4 1106– 23 [Google Scholar]
- Super CM , Harkness S. 2002 . Culture structures the environment for development. Hum. Dev. 45 : 4 270– 74 [Google Scholar]
- Surrain S. 2018 . ‘Spanish at home, English at school’: how perceptions of bilingualism shape family language policies among Spanish-speaking parents of preschoolers. Int. J. Biling. Educ. Biling. 2018 : 1– 15 [Google Scholar]
- Suskind DL , Leffel KR , Graf E , Hernandez MW , Gunderson EA et al. 2016 . A parent-directed language intervention for children of low socioeconomic status: a randomized controlled pilot study. J. Child Lang. 43 : 2 366– 406 [Google Scholar]
- Tamis-LeMonda CS , Custode S , Kuchirko Y , Escobar K , Lo T 2019 . Routine language: speech directed to infants during home activities. Child Dev 90 : 6 2135– 52 [Google Scholar]
- Tamis-LeMonda CS , Song L , Leavell AS , Kahana-Kalman R , Yoshikawa H 2012 . Ethnic differences in mother-infant language and gestural communications are associated with specific skills in infants. Dev. Sci. 15 : 3 384– 97 [Google Scholar]
- Taverna AS , Waxman SR. 2020 . Early lexical acquisition in the Wichi language. J. Child Lang. 2020 : 1– 21 [Google Scholar]
- Thiessen ED. 2011 . Domain general constraints on statistical learning. Child Dev 82 : 2 462– 70 [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello M. 2000 . First steps toward a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cogn. Linguist. 11 : 1–2 61– 82 [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello M , Farrar MJ. 1986 . Joint attention and early language. Child Dev 57 : 6 1454– 63 [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello M , Mannle S. 1985 . Pragmatics of sibling speech to one-year-olds. Child Dev 56 : 4 911– 17 [Google Scholar]
- Tsao FM , Liu HM , Kuhl PK 2004 . Speech perception in infancy predicts language development in the second year of life: a longitudinal study. Child Dev 75 : 4 1067– 84 [Google Scholar]
- Valdés G. 1997 . Dual-language immersion programs: a cautionary note concerning the education of language-minority students. Harv. Educ. Rev. 67 : 3 391– 430 [Google Scholar]
- Vélez-Agosto NM , Soto-Crespo JG , Vizcarrondo-Oppenheimer M , Vega-Molina S , García Coll C 2017 . Bronfenbrenner's bioecological theory revision: moving culture from the macro into the micro. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12 : 5 900– 10 [Google Scholar]
- Vernon-Feagans L , Pancsofar N , Willoughby M 2008 . Predictors of maternal language to infants during a picture book task in the home: family SES, child characteristics and the parenting environment. J. App. Dev. Psychol. 29 : 3 213– 26 [Google Scholar]
- Vogt P , Mastin J. 2013 . Rural and urban differences in language socialization and early vocabulary development in Mozambique. Proc. Ann. Meet. Cogn. Sci. Soc. 35 : 3687– 92 [Google Scholar]
- Warlaumont AS , Richards JA , Gilkerson J , Oller DK 2014 . A social feedback loop for speech development and its reduction in autism. Psychol. Sci. 25 : 7 1314– 24 [Google Scholar]
- Warren-Leubecker A , Bohannon JN. 1984 . Intonation patterns in child-directed speech: mother-father differences. Child Dev 55 : 4 1379– 85 [Google Scholar]
- Weber A , Fernald A , Diop Y 2017 . When cultural norms discourage talking to babies: effectiveness of a parenting program in rural Senegal. Child Dev 88 : 5 1513– 26 [Google Scholar]
- Weisleder A , Cates CB , Harding JF , Johnson SB , Canfield CF et al. 2019 . Links between shared reading and play, parent psychosocial functioning, and child behavior: evidence from a randomized controlled trial. J. Pediatr. 213 : 187– 95 [Google Scholar]
- Weisleder A , Fernald A. 2013 . Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychol. Sci. 24 : 11 2143– 52 [Google Scholar]
- Weisleder A , Fernald A. 2014 . Social environments shape children's language experiences, strengthening language processing and building vocabulary. Language in Interaction. Studies in Honor of Eve V. Clark I Arnon, M Casillas, C Kurumada, B Estigarribia 29– 49 Amsterdam: John Benjamins [Google Scholar]
- Weisleder A , Marchman VA. 2018 . How socioeconomic differences in early language environments shape children's language development. Handbook of Communication Disorders: Theoretical, Empirical, and Applied Linguistics Perspectives A Bar-On, D Ravid 545– 64 Boston, MA: De Gruyter Mouton [Google Scholar]
- Weisleder A , Mazzuchelli DSR , Lopez AS , Neto WD , Cates CB et al. 2018 . Reading aloud and child development: a cluster-randomized trial in Brazil. Pediatrics 141 : e20170723 [Google Scholar]
- Werker JF , Hensch TK. 2015 . Critical periods in speech perception: new directions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 66 : 173– 96 [Google Scholar]
- Winsler A , Díaz RM , Espinosa L , Rodríguez JL 1999 . When learning a second language does not mean losing the first: bilingual language development in low‐income, Spanish‐speaking children attending bilingual preschool. Child Dev 70 : 2 349– 62 [Google Scholar]
- Wong K , Thomas C , Boben M 2020 . Providence talks: a citywide partnership to address early childhood language development. Stud. Educ. Eval. 64 : 100818 [Google Scholar]
- Wong Fillmore L. 1991 . When learning a second language means losing the first. Early Child. Res. Q. 6 : 3 323– 46 [Google Scholar]
- Yu C , Smith LB. 2012 . Embodied attention and word learning by toddlers. Cognition 125 : 2 244– 62 [Google Scholar]
- Yu C , Suanda SH , Smith LB 2018 . Infant sustained attention but not joint attention to objects at 9 months predicts vocabulary at 12 and 15 months. Dev. Sci. 22 : e12735 [Google Scholar]
- Yurovsky D. 2018 . A communicative approach to early word learning. New Ideas Psychol 50 : 73– 79 [Google Scholar]
- Zhang Y , Xu X , Jiang F , Gilkerson J , Xu D et al. 2015 . Effects of quantitative linguistic feedback to caregivers of young children. Commun. Disord. Q. 37 : 1 16– 24 [Google Scholar]
- Article Type: Review Article
Most Read This Month
Most cited most cited rss feed, childhood adversity and neural development: a systematic review, adolescent–parent relationships: progress, processes, and prospects, the life course consequences of very preterm birth, cognitive aging and dementia: a life-span perspective, media and the development of gender role stereotypes, development of adhd: etiology, heterogeneity, and early life course, the development of social categorization, screen time, social media use, and adolescent development, neighborhood effects on children's development in experimental and nonexperimental research.
Cognition and Young Learners’ Language Development
- Living reference work entry
- First Online: 20 October 2020
- Cite this living reference work entry
- Yuko Goto Butler 2
Part of the book series: Springer International Handbooks of Education ((SIHE))
165 Accesses
1 Citations
Because young language learners (YLLs) are in the midst of cognitive development while they are developing their languages, it is important for educators of YLLs to understand children’s cognitive abilities in relation to their linguistic development in order to develop appropriate pedagogy for them. This chapter offers a current understanding of major cognitive abilities that are associated with YLL’s language learning. After sketching main theoretical approaches in general cognitive development, the chapter discusses major research findings concerning the potential influence of bilingualism in cognition/metacognition in the three most studied domains; namely, executive function, theory of mind, and metalinguistic awareness. The chapter also covers emerging neuropsycholinguistic research as well as studies concerning greater use of digital technology by children and its potential impact on cognition. The chapter pays special attention to the kinds of measurements that researchers have used to capture YLLs’ cognitive abilities, and the roles of linguistic inputs and other environmental factors (e.g., socioeconomic status). Although research indicates that there is certainly an association between bilingualism and cognition, there are also substantial variabilities in that association across studies. The underlying mechanisms explaining the nature of the relationships and developmental trajectories are not totally clear. The chapter concludes with suggestions for future research.
This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.
Access this chapter
Institutional subscriptions
Similar content being viewed by others
Effect of Early Bilingualism on Metalinguistic Development and Language Processing: Evidence from Chinese-Speaking Bilingual Children
Cognition and Second Language Experience: How Are Executive Function and Second Language Acquisition Related?
Adesope, O. O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., & Ungerleider, C. (2010). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research, 80 (2), 207–245. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654310368803 .
Article Google Scholar
Alibali, M. W., & Sidney, P. G. (2015). The role of intraindividual variability in learning and cognitive development. In M. Diehl, K. Hooker, & M. J. Sliwinski (Eds.), Handbook of intraindividual variability across the life span (pp. 84–102). New York: Routledge.
Google Scholar
American Psychological Association. (2019). Children, youth, families and socioeconomic status. Retrieved from https://www.apa.org/pi/ses/resources/publications/children-families
Anastasi, A., & Cordova, F. (1953). Some effects of bilingualism upon the intelligence test performance of Puerto Rican children in New York City. Journal of Educational Psychology, 44 , 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0061639 .
Anderson, D. R., & Pempek, T. A. (2005). Television and very young children. American Behavioral Scientist, 48 (5), 505–522. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002764204271506 .
Apel, K. (2014). A comprehensive definition of morphological awareness. Topics in Language Disorders, 34 (3), 197–209. https://doi.org/10.1097/tld.0000000000000019 .
Barac, R., Bialystok, E., Castro, D. C., & Sanchez, M. (2014). The cognitive development of young dual language learners: A critical review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29 (4), 699–714. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.02.003 .
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21 , 37–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8 .
Barr, R. (2013). Memory constraints on infant learning from picture books, television, and touchscreens. Child Development Perspectives, 7 (4), 205–210. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12041 .
Barrouillet, P. (2015). Theories of cognitive development: From Piaget to today. Developmental Review, 38 , 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2015.07.004 .
Belpaeme, T., Vogt, P., van den Berghe, R., Bergmann, K., Göksun, T., de Haas, M., … Pandey, A. K. (2018). Guidelines for designing social robots for second language tutoring. International Journal of Social Robotics , 10 , 325–341. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-018-0467-6 .
Berken, J. A., Gracco, V. L., & Klein, D. (2017). Early bilingualism, language attainment, and brain development. Neuropsychologia, 98 , 220–227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.08.031 .
Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81 , 1641–1660. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x .
Bialystok, E. (1994). Analysis and control in the development of second language proficiency. Studies of Second Language Acquisition, 16 , 157–168. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0272263100012857 .
Bialystok, E. (2018). Bilingualism and executive function. In D. Millder, F. Bayram, J. Rothman, & L. Serratrice (Eds.), Bilingual cognition and language: The state of the science across its subfields (pp. 283–305). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Chapter Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Senman, L. (2004). Executive processes in appearance-reality tasks: The role of inhibition of attention and symbolic representation. Child Development, 75 (2), 562–579. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00693.x .
Bialystok, E., Majumder, S., & Martin, M. (2003). Developing phonological awareness: Is there a bilingual advantage? Applied Psycholinguistics, 24 , 27–44.
Borghi, A. M., & Fini, C. (2019). Theories and explanations in psychology. Frontiers in Psychology, 10 , 598. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00958 .
Bosseler, A. N., Taulu, S., Pihko, E., Mäkelä, J., Imada, T., Ahonen, A., & Kuhl, P. K. (2013). Theta brain rhythms index perceptual narrowing in infant speech perception. Frontiers in Psychology, 4 , 690. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00690 .
Burns, T. C., Yoshida, K. A., Hill, K., & Werker, J. F. (2007). The development of phonetic representation in bilingual and monolingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28 , 455–474. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716407070257 .
Bus, A. G., Takacs, Z. K., & Kegel, C. A. (2015). Affordances and limitations of electronic storybooks for young children’s emergent literacy. Developmental Review, 35 , 79–97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2014.12.004 .
Butler, Y. G. (2017). The role of affect in intraindividual variability in task performance for young learners. TESOL Quarterly, 51 (3), 728–737. https://doi.org/10.1002/tesq.385 .
Butler, Y. G. (2018). Gaming and young learners. In S. Garton & F. Copland (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of teaching English to young learners (pp. 305–319). Oxford: Routledge.
Butler, Y. G. (2019). Teaching vocabulary to young second- or foreign-language learners: What can we learn from the research? Language Teaching for Young Learners, 1 (1), 4–33. https://doi.org/10.1075/ltyl.00003.but .
Butler, Y. G., & Liu, Y. (2019). The role of peers in young learners’ English learning: A longitudinal case study in China. In M. Sato & S. Loewen (Eds.), Evidence-based second language pedagogy: A collection of instructed second language acquisition studies (pp. 145–167). New York: Routledge.
Calkins, S. D., & Bell, M. A. (2010). Introduction: Putting the domains of development into perspective. In S. D. Calkins & M. A. Bell (Eds.), Child development at the intersection of emotion and cognition (pp. 3–13). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, M. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11 , 282–298. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.x .
Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72 (4), 1032–1053. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00333 .
Chapman, M. (1987). Piaget, attention capacity, and the functional limitations of formal structure. Advances in Child Development and Behaviour, 20 , 289–334. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0065-2407(08)60405-x .
Chen, X., & Schwartz, M. (2018). Morphological awareness and literacy in second language learners: A cross-language perspective. Reading and Writing, 31 (8), 1685–1694. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-018-9866-1 .
Cheung, H., Mak, W. Y., Luo, X., & Xiao, W. (2010). Sociolinguistic awareness and false belief in young Cantonese learners of English. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107 , 188–194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.05.001 .
Clark, D. B., Tanner-Smith, E. E., & Killingworth, S. S. (2016). Digital games, design, and learning: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 86 (1), 79–122. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654315582065 .
Conboy, B. T., & Kuhl, P. K. (2011). Impact of second-language experience in infancy: Brain measures of first- and second-language speech perception. Developmental Science, 14 (2), 242–248. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00973.x .
David, A., & Wei, L. (2008). Individual differences in the lexical development of French–English bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 11 (5), 598–618. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050802149200 .
Davidson, D., Raschke, V. R., & Pervez, J. (2010). Syntactic awareness in young monolingual and bilingual (Urdu–English) children. Cognitive Development, 25 , 166–182. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.07.003 .
de Bot, K. (2017). Complexity theory and dynamic systems theory: In celebration of Diane Larsen-Freeman. In L. Ortega & Z. Han (Eds.), Complexity theory and language development (pp. 51–58). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
de Bruin, A., Treccani, B., & Sala, S. D. (2015). Cognitive advantage in bilingualism: An example of publication bias? Psychological Science, 26 (1), 99–107. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614557866 .
de Cat, C., Gusnanto, A., & Serratrice, L. (2018). Identifying a threshold for the executive function advantage in bilingual children. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40 , 119–151. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0272263116000486 .
Deák, G. O. (2014). Interrelationship of language and cognitive development (overview). In P. Brooks & V. Kampe (Eds.), Encyclopedia of language development (pp. 284–291). Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.
Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2014). Relations between false beliefs understanding and executive function in early childhood: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 85 (5), 1777–1794. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12237 .
Diamond, A. (2006). The early development of executive functions. In E. Bialystok & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Lifespan cognition: Mechanisms of change (pp. 70–95). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64 , 135–168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750 .
Diaz, V., & Farrar, M. J. (2018). The missing explanation of the false-belief advantage in bilingual children: A longitudinal study. Developmental Science, 21 . https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12594 .
Dixon, L. Q., Chuang, H., & Quiroz, B. (2012). English phonological awareness in bilinguals; a cross-linguistic study of Tamil, Malay and Chinese English-language learners. Journal of Research in Reading, 35 (4), 372–392. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01471.x .
Donaldson, M. (1978). Children’s minds . Glasgow: Fontana.
Engel de Abreu, P., Cruz-Santos, A., Tourinho, C. J., Martin, R., & Bialystok, E. (2012). Bilingualism enriches the poor: Enhanced cognitive control in low-income minority children. Psychological Science, 23 (11), 1364–1371. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612443836 .
Erickson, L. C., & Thiessen, E. D. (2015). Statistical learning of language: Theory, validity, and predictions of a statistical learning account of language acquisition. Developmental Review, 37 , 66–108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2015.05.002 .
Eviatar, Z., Taha, H., & Schwartz, M. (2018). Metalinguistic awareness and literacy among semitic-bilingual learners: A cross-language perspective. Reading and Writing, 31 (8), 1869–1891. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-018-9850-9 .
Fan, S. P., Liberman, Z., Keysar, B., & Kinzler, K. D. (2015). The exposure advantage: Early exposure to a multilingual environment promotes effective communication. Psychological Science, 26 (7), 1090–1097. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615574699 .
Farhadian, M., Abdullah, R., Mansor, M., Redzuan, M., Gazanizadand, N., & Kumar, V. (2010). Theory of mind in bilingual and monolingual preschool children. Journal of Psychology, 1 , 39–46. https://doi.org/10.1080/09764224.2010.11885444 .
Ferguson, C. J., & Donnellan, M. B. (2014). Is the association between children’s baby video viewing and poor language development robust? A reanalysis of Zimmerman, Christakis, and Meltzoff (2007). Developmental Psychology, 50 (1), 129–137. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033628 .
Foursha-Stevenson, C., & Nicoladis, E. (2011). Early emergence of syntactic awareness and cross-linguistic influence in bilingual children’s judgments. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15 (4), 521–534. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006911425818 .
Gajdamaschko, N. (2015). Vygotsky’s sociocultural theory. In J. Wright (Ed.), International encyclopedia of the social & behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Amsterdam: Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.23203-0 .
Goetz, P. (2003). The effects of bilingualism on theory of mind development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6 , 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1366728903001007 .
Gooch, D., Thompson, P., Nash, H. M., Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (2016). The development of executive function and language skills in the early school years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57 (2), 180–187. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12458 .
Gopnik, A., & Bonawitz, E. (2015). Bayesian models of child development. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 6 , 75–86. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1330 .
Grundy, J. G., & Timmer, K. (2017). Bilingualism and working memory capacity: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Second Language Research, 33 (3), 325–340. https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658316678286 .
Halford, G. S., & Andrews, G. (2015). Information-processing models of cognitive development. In U. Goswami (Ed.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 697–722). Malden: Wiley.
Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H., & Philips, S. (1998). Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21 , 803–831. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x98001769 .
Kail, R. (1986). Sources of age differences in speed of processing. Child Development, 57 , 969–987. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130372 .
Koda, K. (2005). Insights into second language reading: A cross-linguistic approach . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Book Google Scholar
Kovács, A. M. (2009). Early bilingualism enhances mechanisms of false-belief reasoning. Developmental Science, 12 (1), 48–54. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00742.x .
Kozulin, A. (2015). Vygotsky’s theory of cognitive development. In J. Wright (Ed.), International encyclopedia of the social & behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Amsterdam: Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.23094 .
Krizman, J., Marian, V., Shook, A., Skoe, E., & Kraus, N. (2012). Subcortical encoding of sounds is enhanced in bilinguals and relates to executive function advantages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109 (20), 7877–7887.
Kuhl, P. K. (2010). Brain mechanism in early language acquisition. Neuron, 67 (5), 713–727. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.038 .
Kuhl, P. K. (2011). Social mechanisms in early language acquisition: Understanding integrated brain systems supporting language. In J. Decety & J. Cacioppo (Eds.), The handbook of social neuroscience (pp. 649–667). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Kuo, L., & Anderson, R. C. (2008). Conceptual and methodological issues in comparing metalinguistic awareness across languages. In K. Koda & A. M. Zehler (Eds.), Learning to read across languages: Cross-linguistic relationships in first- and second-language literacy development (pp. 39–67). New York: Routledge.
Ladas, A. I., Carroll, D. J., & Vivas, A. B. (2015). Attentional processes in low-socioeconomic status bilingual children: Are they modulated by the amount of bilingual experience? Child Development, 86 (2), 557–578. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12332 .
Lantolf, J. P. (Ed.). (2000). Sociocultural theory and second language learning . Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Larsen-Freeman, D. (2011). A complexity theory approach to second language development/acquisition. In D. Atkinson (Ed.), Alternative approaches to second language acquisition (pp. 48–72). London: Routledge.
Lawson, G. M., Hook, C. J., & Farah, M. J. (2018). A meta-analysis of the relationship between socioeconomic status and executive function among children. Developmental Science, 21 . https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12529 .
Lewis, M. D. (2000). The promise of dynamic systems approaches for an integrated account of human development. Child Development, 71 , 36–43. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00116 .
Liberman, Z., Woodward, A. L., Keysar, B., & Kinzler, K. D. (2017). Exposure to multiple languages enhances communication skills in infancy. Developmental Science, 20 . https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12420 .
Liu, D., Wellman, H., Tardif, T., & Sabbagh, M. (2008). Theory of mind development among Chinese children: A meta-analysis of false-belief understanding across cultures and languages. Developmental Psychology, 44 , 523–531. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.523 .
Loizou, M., & Stuart, M. (2003). Phonological awareness in monolingual and bilingual English and Greek five-year-olds. Journal of Research in Reading, 26 (1), 3–18. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.261002 .
MacLeod, C. M. (2015). Attention: Beyond Stroop’s (1935) colour-word interference phenomenon. In M. W. Eysenck & D. Groome (Eds.), Cognitive psychology: Revising the classic studies (pp. 60–70). London: Sage.
Marchman, V. A., Martínez, L. Z., Hurtado, N., Grüter, T., & Fernald, A. (2017). Caregiver talk to young Spanish–English bilinguals: Comparing direct observation and parent-report measures of dual-language exposure. Developmental Science, 20 . https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12425 .
Mascolo, M. F. (2015). Neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development. In J. Wright (Ed.), International encyclopedia of the social & behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Amsterdam: Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.23097-3 .
Miller, P. H. (2014). Piaget’s theory: Past, present, and future. In U. Goswami (Ed.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 649–672). Malden: Wiley.
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21 (1), 8–14. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411429458 .
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41 , 49–100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734 .
Mohades, S. G., Struys, E., Von Schuerbeek, P., Mondt, K., Van De Craen, P., & Luypaert, R. (2012). DTI reveals structural differences in white matter tracts between bilingual and monolingual children. Brain Research, 1435 , 72–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2011.12.005 .
Moll, L. C., Amanti, C., Neff, D., & Gonzalez, N. (1992). Funds of knowledge for teaching: Using a qualitative approach to connect homes and classrooms. Theory Into Practice, 31 (2), 132–141. https://doi.org/10.1080/00405849209543534 .
Murakami, A. (2016). Modeling systematicity and individuality in nonlinear second language development: The case of English grammatical morphemes. Language Learning, 66 (4), 834–871. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12166 .
Nathanson, A. I., Aladé, F., Sharp, M. L., Rasmussen, E. E., & Christy, K. (2014). The relation between television exposure and executive function among preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 50 (5), 1497–1506. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035714 .
Nguyen, T., & Astington, J. W. (2014). Reassessing the bilingual advantage in theory of mind and its cognitive underpinnings. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17 (2), 396–409. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1366728913000394 .
Oatley, K., Parrott, W. G., Smith, C., & Watts, F. (2011). Cognition and emotion over twenty-five years. Cognition and Emotion, 25 (8), 1341–1348. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2011.622949 .
Paap, K. R., Anders-Jefferson, R., Wason, L., Alvarado, K., & Zimiga, B. (2018). Bilingual advantages in inhibition or selective attention: More challenges. Frontiers in Psychology, 9 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01409 .
Paradis, J. (2011). Individual differences in child English second language acquisition: Comparing child internal and child external factors. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1 , 213–237. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.1.3.01par .
Peal, E., & Lambert, W. E. (1962). The relation of bilingualism to intelligence. Psychological Monographs, 76 , 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093840 .
Pearl, L., & Goldwater, S. (2016). Statistical learning, inductive bias, and Bayesian inference in language acquisition. In J. L. Lidz, W. Snyder, & J. Pater (Eds.), Oxford handbook of developmental linguistics . Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199601264.013.28 .
Piaget, J. (1952). Origins of intelligence in the child (trans: Cook, A.). New York: International Universities Press. (Original Universities Press work published 1936).
Pierce, L. J., Genesee, F., Delcenserie, A., & Morgan, G. (2017). Variations in phonological working memory: Linking early language experiences and language learning outcomes. Applied Psycholinguistics, 38 , 1265–1300. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716417000236 .
Raviv, L., & Arnon, I. (2018). The developmental trajectory of children’s auditory and visual statistical learning abilities: Modality-based differences in the effect of age. Developmental Science, 21 . https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12593 .
Reid Chassiakos, Y., Radesky, J., Christakis, D., Moreno, M. A., & Cross, C. (2016). Children and adolescents and digital media. Pediatrics, 138 (5), e20162593.
Rolla San Francisco, A. R., Carlo, M., August, D., & Snow, C. (2006). The role of language of instruction and vocabulary in the English phonological awareness of Spanish-English bilingual children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27 (2), 229–246. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716406060267 .
Romberg, A. R., & Saffran, J. R. (2010). Statistical learning and language acquisition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1 (6), 906–914.
Romeo, R. R., Leonard, J. A., Robinson, S. T., West, M. R., Mackey, A. P., Rowe, M. L., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2018). Beyond the 30-million-word gap: Children’s conversational exposure is associated with language-related brain function. Psychological Science, 29 (5), 700–710. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617742725 .
Sabourin, L., & Vīnerte, S. (2015). The bilingual advantage in the Stroop task: Simultaneous vs. early bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18 (2), 350–355. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1366728914000704 .
Saito, D., Tanabe, H., Izuma, K., Hayashi, M., Morito, Y., Komeda, H., … Sadato, N. (2010). “Stay tuned”: Inter-individual neural synchronization during mutual gaze and join attention. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience , 4 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2010.00127 .
Schwarz, M., & Shaul, S. (2018). Towards a better understanding of the link between executive functions and pre-academic abilities. Written Language and Literacy, 21 (2), 238–268. https://doi.org/10.1075/wll.00016.sch .
Schwarz, M., Taha, H., Assad, H., Khamaisi, F., & Eviatar, Z. (2016). The role of emergent bilingualism in the development of morphological awareness in Arabic and Hebrew. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 59 , 797–809. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_jslhr-l-14-0363 .
Shahaeian, A., Peterson, C. C., Slaughter, V., & Wellman, H. M. (2011). Culture and the sequence of steps in theory of mind development. Developmental Psychology, 47 (5), 1239–1247. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023899 .
Šimleša, S., & Cepanec, M. (2015). Development of executive functions during childhood. In J. Wright (Ed.), International encyclopedia of the social & behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Amsterdam: Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.23083-3 .
Šimleša, S., Cepanec, M., & Ljubešić, M. (2017). The role of executive functions in language comprehension in preschool children. Psychology, 8 , 227–245. https://doi.org/10.4236/psych.2017.82013 .
Smith, M. E. (1939). Some light on the problem of bilingualism as found from a study of the progress in mastery of English among preschool children of non-American ancestry in Hawaii. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 21 , 121–284.
Sorge, G. B., Toplak, M. E., & Bialystok, E. (2017). Interactions between levels of attention ability and levels of bilingualism in children’s executive functioning. Developmental Science, 20 . https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12408 .
Strouse, G. A., O’Doherty, K., & Troseth, G. L. (2013). Effective coviewing: Preschoolers’ learning from video after a dialogic questioning intervention. Developmental Psychology, 49 (12), 2368–2382. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032463 .
Sun, H., Yin, B., Amsah, F., & O’Brien, B. A. (2018). Differential effects of internal and external factors in early bilingual vocabulary learning: The case of Singapore. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39 (2), 383–411. https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271641700039X .
Sun, H., Loh, J. Y., & Charles, A. C. (2019). Motion and sound in animated storybooks for preschooler’s total fixation time and mandarin language learning: An eye-tracking study with Singaporean bilingual children. AERA Open, 5 , 2), 1–2),19. https://doi.org/10.1177/2332858419848431 .
Takanishi, R., & Le Menestrel, S. (2017). Promoting educational success of children and youth learning English: Promising futures . Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
Thomas-Sunesson, D., Hakuta, K., & Bialystok, E. (2018). Degree of bilingualism modifies executive control in Hispanic children in the USA. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 21 (2), 197–206. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2016.1148114 .
Valian, V. (2015). Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18 (1), 3–24. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1366728914000522 .
Verga, L., & Kotz, S. A. (2013). How relevant is social interaction in second language learning? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00550 .
Vogt, P., de Hass, M., de Jong, C., Baxter, P., & Krahmer, E. (2017). Child-robot interactions for second language tutoring to preschool children. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00073 .
Vulchanova, M., Baggio, G., Cangelosi, A., & Smith, L. (2017). Editorial: Language development in the digital age. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00447 .
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes (Eds. and trans: Cole, M., John-Steiner, V., Scribner, S., & Souberman, E.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Original work published 1930).
Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language (Ed. and trans: Kozulin, A.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Original work published 1934).
Wellman, H. M. (2014). Developing a theory of mind. In U. Goswami (Ed.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 258–284). Malden: Wiley.
White, L. J., & Greenfield, D. B. (2017). Executive functioning in Spanish- and English-speaking head start preschoolers. Developmental Science, 20 . https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12502 .
Woodard, E., & Rodman, A. (2007). Socio-economic status as a mediating factor in the relationship between bilingualism and the cognitive development of pre-school children . Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Communication Association 93rd annual convention.
Wouters, P., van Nimwegen, C., van Oostendorp, H., & van der Spek, E. D. (2013). A meta-analysis of the cognitive and motivational effects of serious games. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105 (2), 249–265. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031311 .
Yoshioka, J. G. (1929). A study of bilingualism. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 36 , 473–479.
Zelazo, P. D., & Müller, U. (2014). Executive function in typical and atypical development. In U. Goswami (Ed.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 574–603). Malden: Wiley.
Zelazo, P. D., Müller, U., Frye, D., & Marcovitch, S. (2003). The development of executive function. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68 (3), 1–115.
Zhang, Y., Song, H., Liu, X., Tang, D., Chen, Y., & Zhang, X. (2017). Language learning enhanced by massive multiple online role-playing games (MMORPGs) and the underlying behavioral and neural mechanism. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00095 .
Zimmerman, F. J., Christakis, D. A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). Associations between media viewing and language development in children under age two years. The Journal of Pediatrics, 151 , 364–368. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2007.04.071 .
Download references
Author information
Authors and affiliations.
Educational Linguistics Division, University of Pennsylvania Graduate School of Education, Philadelphia, PA, USA
Yuko Goto Butler
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
Corresponding author
Correspondence to Yuko Goto Butler .
Editor information
Editors and affiliations.
Language Department, Oranim Academic College of Education, Kiriat Tivon, Israel
Mila Schwartz
Rights and permissions
Reprints and permissions
Copyright information
© 2020 Springer Nature Switzerland AG
About this entry
Cite this entry.
Butler, Y.G. (2020). Cognition and Young Learners’ Language Development. In: Schwartz, M. (eds) Handbook of Early Language Education. Springer International Handbooks of Education. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-47073-9_2-1
Download citation
DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-47073-9_2-1
Received : 29 June 2020
Accepted : 29 June 2020
Published : 20 October 2020
Publisher Name : Springer, Cham
Print ISBN : 978-3-030-47073-9
Online ISBN : 978-3-030-47073-9
eBook Packages : Springer Reference Education Reference Module Humanities and Social Sciences Reference Module Education
- Publish with us
Policies and ethics
- Find a journal
- Track your research
- NAEYC Login
- Member Profile
- Hello Community
- Accreditation Portal
- Online Learning
- Online Store
Popular Searches: DAP ; Coping with COVID-19 ; E-books ; Anti-Bias Education ; Online Store
Language and Literacy Development: Research-Based, Teacher-Tested Strategies
You are here
“Why does it tick and why does it tock?”
“Why don’t we call it a granddaughter clock?”
“Why are there pointy things stuck to a rose?”
“Why are there hairs up inside of your nose?”
She started with Why? and then What? How? and When? By bedtime she came back to Why? once again. She drifted to sleep as her dazed parents smiled at the curious thoughts of their curious child, who wanted to know what the world was about. They kissed her and whispered, “You’ll figure it out.”
—Andrea Beaty, Ada Twist, Scientist
I have dozens of favorite children’s books, but while working on this cluster about language and literacy development, Ada Twist, Scientist kept coming to mind. Ada is an African American girl who depicts the very essence of what it means to be a scientist. The book is a celebration of children’s curiosity, wonder, and desire to learn.
The more I thought about language and literacy, the more Ada became my model. All children should have books as good as Ada Twist, Scientist read to them. All children should be able to read books like Ada Twist, Scientist by the end of third grade. All children should be encouraged to ask questions about their world and be supported in developing the literacy tools (along with broad knowledge, inquiring minds, and other tools!) to answer those questions. All children should see themselves in books that rejoice in learning.
Early childhood teachers play a key role as children develop literacy. While this cluster does not cover the basics of reading instruction, it offers classroom-tested ways to make common practices like read alouds and discussions even more effective.
The cluster begins with “ Enhancing Toddlers’ Communication Skills: Partnerships with Speech-Language Pathologists ,” by Janet L. Gooch. In a mutually beneficial partnership, interns from a university communication disorders program supported Early Head Start teachers in learning several effective ways to boost toddlers’ language development, such as modeling the use of new vocabulary and expanding on what toddlers say. (One quirk of Ada Twist, Scientist is that Ada doesn’t speak until she is 3; in real life, that would be cause for significant concern. Having a submission about early speech interventions was pure serendipity.) Focusing on preschoolers, Kathleen M. Horst, Lisa H. Stewart, and Susan True offer a framework for enhancing social, emotional, and academic learning. In “ Joyful Learning with Stories: Making the Most of Read Alouds ,” they explain how to establish emotionally supportive routines that are attentive to each child’s strengths and needs while also increasing group discussions. During three to five read alouds of a book, teachers engage children in building knowledge, vocabulary, phonological awareness, and concepts of print.
Next up, readers go inside the lab school at Stepping Stones Museum for Children. In “ Equalizing Opportunities to Learn: A Collaborative Approach to Language and Literacy Development in Preschool ,” Laura B. Raynolds, Margie B. Gillis, Cristina Matos, and Kate Delli Carpini share the engaging, challenging activities they designed with and for preschoolers growing up in an under-resourced community. Devondre finds out how hard Michelangelo had to work to paint the ceiling of the Sistine Chapel, and Sayo serves as a guide in the children’s classroom minimuseum— taking visitors to her artwork!
Moving into first grade, Laura Beth Kelly, Meridith K. Ogden, and Lindsey Moses explain how they helped children learn to lead and participate in meaningful discussions of literature. “ Collaborative Conversations: Speaking and Listening in the Primary Grades ” details the children’s progress (and the teacher’s methods) as they developed discussion-related social and academic skills. Although the first graders still required some teacher facilitation at the end of the school year, they made great strides in preparing for conversations, listening to their peers, extending others’ comments, asking questions, and reflecting on discussions.
Rounding out the cluster are two articles on different aspects of learning to read. In “ Sounding It Out Is Just the First Step: Supporting Young Readers ,” Sharon Ruth Gill briefly explains the complexity of the English language and suggests several ways teachers can support children as they learn to decode fluently. Her tips include giving children time to self-correct, helping them use semantic and syntactic cues, and analyzing children’s miscues to decide what to teach next.
In “ Climbing Fry’s Mountain: A Home–School Partnership for Learning Sight Words ,” Lynda M. Valerie and Kathleen A. Simoneau describe a fun program for families. With game-like activities that require only basic household items, children in kindergarten through second grade practice reading 300 sight words. Children feel successful as they begin reading, and teachers reserve instructional time for phonological awareness, phonics, vocabulary, and other essentials of early reading.
At the end of Ada Twist, Scientist , there is a marvelous illustration of Ada’s whole family reading. “They remade their world—now they’re all in the act / of helping young Ada sort fiction from fact.” It reminds me of the power of reading and of the important language and literacy work that early childhood educators do every day.
—Lisa Hansel
We’d love to hear from you!
Send your thoughts on this issue, as well as topics you’d like to read about in future issues of Young Children , to [email protected] .
Would you like to see your children’s artwork featured? For guidance on submitting print-quality photos (as well as details on permissions and licensing), see NAEYC.org/resources/pubs/authors-photographers/photos .
Is your classroom full of children’s artwork? To feature it in Young Children , see the link at the bottom of the page or email [email protected] for details.
Lisa Hansel, EdD, is the editor in chief of NAEYC's peer-reviewed journal, Young Children .
Vol. 74, No. 1
Print this article
An official website of the United States government
The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.
The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.
- Publications
- Account settings
The PMC website is updating on October 15, 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .
- Advanced Search
- Journal List
- HHS Author Manuscripts
Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary
Associated data.
Infants differ substantially in their rates of language growth, and slower growth predicts later academic difficulties. This study explored how the amount of speech to infants in Spanish-speaking families low in socioeconomic status (SES) influenced the development of children's skill in real-time language processing and vocabulary learning. All-day recordings of parent-infant interactions at home revealed striking variability among families in how much speech caregivers addressed to their child. Infants who experienced more child-directed speech became more efficient in processing familiar words in real time and had larger expressive vocabularies by 24 months, although speech simply overheard by the child was unrelated to vocabulary outcomes. Mediation analyses showed that the effect of child-directed speech on expressive vocabulary was explained by infants’ language-processing efficiency, suggesting that richer language experience strengthens processing skills that facilitate language growth.
At any given age, there is wide variability among children in their levels of language proficiency ( Fenson et al., 1994 ). Although differences in verbal abilities among individuals are influenced to some extent by genetic factors ( Oliver & Plomin, 2007 ), the contributions of early experience to such differences are also substantial. Factors associated with socioeconomic status (SES) are strongly associated with variation in language outcomes. By the time they enter kindergarten, children from disadvantaged backgrounds differ significantly from their more advantaged peers in verbal and other cognitive abilities ( Ramey & Ramey, 2004 ), disparities that are predictive of later academic success or failure ( Hart & Risley, 1995 ; Lee & Burkam, 2002 ). Identifying the environmental factors that shape these consequential differences in early language proficiency is critical for remediating the growing achievement gaps between children from impoverished and affluent families ( Duncan & Murnane, 2011 ).
What accounts for differences among children in their early language growth? One source of variability in rates of language learning is differential access to language and gesture from caregivers. Some parents talk more and use richer vocabulary and gesture in interactions with infants than do others, and such differences in the quantity and quality of language input account in part for later disparities among children in lexical and grammatical development, both within and between socio-economic groups ( Hart & Risley, 1995 ; Hoff, 2003a ; Huttenlocher, Waterfall, Vasilyeva, Vevea, & Hedges, 2010 ; Pan, Rowe, Singer, & Snow, 2005 ; Rowe & Goldin-Meadow, 2009 ). A second source of variability in language learning relates to infants’ speech processing skills. Differences among infants in phonological discrimination ( Tsao, Liu, & Kuhl, 2004 ) and spoken word recognition ( Fernald, Perfors, & Marchman, 2006 ; Singh, Steven Reznick, & Xuehua, 2012 ) predict early vocabulary growth. In experimental studies in which infants look at pictures of familiar objects as one object is named, their speed and accuracy in recognizing the object name and identifying the correct picture in real time predicts both early vocabulary development and later language and cognitive skills ( Fernald & Marchman, 2011 ; Fernald et al., 2006 ; Marchman & Fernald, 2008 ).
These studies show that children's language outcomes are linked both to early experience with language and to speech processing skills in infancy, but it is not well understood how these two influences work together over development to promote vocabulary growth. Here we investigate two alternative possibilities: One is that language experience and language-processing skill are separate factors that contribute independently to lexical development. That is, variation in children's vocabulary growth could result from differences in children's exposure to speech – and thus in their opportunities to learn new words – or from pre-existing differences in children's ability to process speech efficiently, with some children better able to take advantage of the learning opportunities available to them.
Another possibility is that early experience with language actually influences the development of efficiency in real-time language processing. That is, experience in hearing language from caregivers may sharpen infants’ skill in processing speech, and hence improve their ability to learn from future language input. A recent study comparing infants from higher- and lower-SES families found that significant disparities in language processing efficiency were already present by 18 months of age ( Fernald, Marchman, & Weisleder, 2013 ), suggesting that experiential factors associated with SES may contribute to differences in processing skill. In addition, one previous study showed that infants exposed to richer language input were more efficient in language processing ( Hurtado, Marchman, & Fernald, 2008 ). However, in this study the relation between language experience and processing efficiency could be explained by children's vocabulary size. To address this gap, we ask here: Is early experience with language linked to the development of efficiency in language processing? And if so, do differences in processing efficiency mediate the well-established relation between early language experience and later vocabulary knowledge? Answers to these questions will help us understand the developmental pathways linking early language experience, speech-processing efficiency and vocabulary growth.
We focus on infants from low-SES Latino families, a rapidly growing population of children in the U.S. at risk for academic difficulties ( Reardon & Galindo, 2009 ). Rather than relying on short samples of mothers’ speech with an observer present ( Hurtado et al., 2008 ; Pan et al., 2005 ), we collected more extensive and representative recordings of infants’ interactions with family members during a typical day at home. We examine how these naturalistic measures of caregiver speech relate to experimental measures of language processing and to parent report of expressive vocabulary.
Participants
Participants were 29 Spanish-learning infants (19 F, 10 M) tested at the ages of 19 and 24 months. Parents reported that all infants were full-term and typically developing. An additional 6 children were excluded from the sample because the home recordings were not conducted properly ( n =3), the computer malfunctioned during testing ( n =2), or the infant received a diagnosis of developmental delay during the course of the study ( n =1). Median family income ranged from < $25,000 to $75,000 per year, with 79% of families reporting a yearly income below the federal poverty line. Although parents varied in years of education, most had not completed high school. Maternal education ranged from 4 – 16 years ( M = 10, SD = 3) and was used as the primary index of SES, controlled in all analyses. All parents were native speakers of Spanish and all children heard Spanish as the primary language in the home, with less than 25% exposure to English from adults or other children.
Measures of the Home Language Environment
To measure adult speech accessible to infants in different families, audio-recordings were made during a typical day at home when the child was 19 months old. A digital recorder in the chest pocket of specialized clothing worn by the child enabled unobtrusive recording of both child-directed and overheard speech in daily interactions among family members ( Ford, Baer, Xu, Yapanel, & Gray, 2009 ). Parents were asked to record their child “during a typical day in the home”, and to keep a logbook indicating the locations in which the recording was conducted, who was present, the main activities the child was engaged in, and whether anything atypical occurred.
Families recorded for an average of 11 hours (range: 4-16) over the course of 1-6 days. Using information recorded in the logbooks, we selected for each family the longest available recording that represented a typical day. Estimates of adult word counts based on these recordings were highly correlated with adult word counts aggregated over all days of recording ( r = .84, p < .001). After eliminating nap times, the final sample of recordings had an average duration of 7 hours (range: 3-13). Differences in the length of these recordings were controlled for in all analyses.
The home recordings were analyzed by LENA analysis software ( Xu, Yapanel, & Gray, 2009 ). This software processes the audio files and yields estimates of different components of the infant's language environment, including the number of adult word tokens and the number of child vocalizations . The accuracy of these estimates for English-language recordings has been established in previous studies ( Xu et al., 2009 ; Oller et al., 2010 ; Oetting, Hartfield & Pruitt, 2009 ). To assess the accuracy of the adult word estimates in Spanish-language environments, 60-min samples from 10 of the current home recordings were transcribed by native Spanish-speakers otherwise uninvolved in this research. This analysis revealed a high correlation between automated estimates of adult words and transcriber-based word counts ( r = .80), confirming that the LENA system provides reliable estimates of adult words in Spanish language environments (further details can be found in the Supporting Methods available online).
To differentiate between speech directed to the child and speech overheard by the child, native Spanish-speaking coders listened to each of the home recordings and classified each 5-min segment as containing speech that was predominantly “child-directed” or “overheard”. The number of adult word tokens in segments classified as child-directed, divided by the duration of the recording, served as our measure of “child-directed speech”; the number of adult word tokens in segments classified as overheard, divided by the duration of the recording, served as our measure of “overheard speech”; the number of speech-like vocalizations produced by the target child in segments classified as child-directed, divided by the duration of the recording, served as our measure of “child vocalizations” (see Supporting Methods for further details).
Measures of Expressive Vocabulary
At 24 months, parents completed the MacArthur-Bates Inventario del Desarrollo de Habilidades Comunicativas: Palabras y Enuciados (Inventario II) ( Jackson-Maldonado et al., 2003 ), the Spanish-language version of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories (MCDI). Productive vocabulary scores were based on the number of words parents reported their child “comprende y dice” (“understands and says”).
Measures of Language-Processing Efficiency
In the “looking-while-listening” (LWL) task ( Fernald, Zangl, Portillo, & Marchman, 2008 ), infants were presented with pairs of images (e.g., a dog and a baby) while hearing sentences naming one of the pictures. Children were tested on words that are frequent in child-directed speech and are familiar to most children in the participants’ age range, based on the MCDI lexical norms ( Dale & Fenson, 1996 ). At 19 months, the eight target nouns were: el perro ‘doggie’ , el libro ‘book’ , el jugo ‘juice’ , el globo ‘balloon’ , el zapato ‘shoe’ , el plátano ‘banana’ , la pelota ‘ball’ , and la galleta ‘cookie’ . At 24 months, four additional familiar words were included: el caballo ‘horse’ , el pájaro ‘bird’ , la cuchara ‘spoon’ , and la manzana ‘apple’ . All of the words were presented in simple sentence frames ending with the target noun (e.g., Mira el perro. ‘Look at the doggie.’).
The speech stimuli were recorded by a native Spanish-speaking adult female and edited for prosodic comparability. Visual stimuli consisted of digital pictures of objects presented in yoked pairs. The pairs were matched for visual salience, the grammatical gender of the object name, and lexical familiarity (based on the MCDI lexical norms; Dale & Fenson, 1996 ). Each object was presented an equal number of times as target or distracter. Table S1 in the Supplemental Material available online lists the word pairs as presented in the experiments at 19 and at 24 months.
Children sat on their parent's lap about 60 cm from the screen, and parents wore opaque sunglasses to block their view of the images. On each trial, two pictures were presented in silence for 2 s, followed by a ca. 3-s speech stimulus, and a 1-s silent period during which the pictures remained on-screen. At 19 months, the 8 target nouns were presented four times each, for a total of 32 test trials; at 24 months, the 12 target nouns were presented three times each, for a total of 36 test trials. Side of target presentation was counter-balanced across trials, and trial-order was counter-balanced across participants. The entire test session lasted 4 - 5 min.
Children's looking patterns were video-recorded. Subsequently, highly trained coders blind to target location coded the child's gaze patterns. On each frame, coders noted whether the child was fixating the left or right pictures, in transition between the two pictures, or looking away from both. A second coder independently re-coded all trials for 28% of the participants at each age. The proportion of frames on which observers agreed within a single frame was 99%.
Speech processing efficiency was calculated as the proportion of time the infant spent fixating the target picture out of total time looking at either the target or distracter picture, within 300-1800 ms from target word onset ( Fernald et al., 2008 ). Only those trials on which the child was looking at either the target or distracter picture at the onset of the target noun were included in these analyses. This measure of efficiency captures children's tendency to shift rapidly toward the target picture when initially looking at the distracter, as well as their tendency to maintain attention to the target when they are already looking at it.
Among these low-SES families, there was striking variability in the total amount of adult speech accessible to the infant, which ranged from almost 29,000 adult words to fewer than 2,000 words over a 10-hour day ( Figure 1 ). When only talk addressed directly to the child was considered, these differences were even more extreme: in one family, caregivers spoke more than 12,000 words to the infant, while in another the infant heard only 670 words of child-directed speech over an entire day, an 18-fold difference in the amount of child-directed speech available to these two children. These differences in parental engagement were uncorrelated with maternal education ( r = .29, p = .13). In addition, amount of child-directed speech was not correlated with amount of overheard speech ( r = .17, p = .38), suggesting that the observed differences in speech to children were not due to overall differences in talkativeness among families, but rather to caregivers’ degree of verbal engagement with their infants.
Variability across 29 families in the amount of adult speech infants heard in a typical day at home. The height of each bar indicates the total number of adult words spoken in proximity to the target child in one family, calculated by averaging the word counts per waking hour and extrapolating to a 10-hr day. The proportion of total words that was child-directed speech is indicated in black, with overheard speech in white.
Links between Language Experience and Vocabulary
We next asked whether differences among families in amount of speech available to infants predicted children's vocabulary six months later. Those children who heard more child-directed speech at 19 months had larger vocabularies at 24 months ( r = 0.57, p < 0.01), consistent with previous findings ( Hoff, 2003a ; Hurtado et al., 2008 ). In contrast, differences in exposure to overheard speech directed to other adults and children were not related to vocabulary size ( r = .25, p = .2), suggesting that language spoken directly to the child is more supportive of early lexical development than speech simply overheard by the child. One alternative possibility is that infants with more precocious language skills tend to vocalize more often, eliciting more speech from their caregivers. If so, and if infants who produce more speech early on have larger productive vocabularies at 24 months, this might account for the relation between child-directed speech and later vocabulary ( Newport, Gleitman, & Gleitman, 1977 ). To examine this possibility, we first analyzed the relation between infant vocalizations and child-directed speech at 19 months. Infants who vocalized more often did hear more child-directed speech ( r = .41, p < .05), suggesting some degree of concordance between infants’ and caregivers’ vocalizations. However, even after controlling for infant vocalizations at 19 months, the relation between child-directed speech and 24-month vocabulary remained robust ( r = 0.51, p < 0.01). This suggests that over and above differences in infants’ expressive language skill early on, exposure to child-directed speech predicted later vocabulary size.
Links between Language Experience and Language Processing
These results support previous findings showing that early language experience predicts later vocabulary knowledge. But are children who hear more child-directed speech also more efficient in processing familiar words in real time? Amount of exposure to child-directed speech was reliably correlated with children's processing efficiency at 19 ( r = 0.44, p < 0.05) and 24 months ( r = 0.51, p < 0.01) ( Figures 2 and and3b 3b illustrate these relations). Moreover, controlling for differences in processing at 19 months, children who heard more child-directed speech were more efficient in language processing at 24 months than those who heard less child-directed speech ( r = 0.47, p < 0.05). This indicates that amount of exposure to child-directed speech explained gains in processing efficiency from 19 to 24 months. Importantly, the relation between child-directed speech and processing efficiency at 24 months remained significant when controlling for differences in 24-month vocabulary size ( r = 0.39, p < 0.05). This indicates that over and above differences in vocabulary knowledge, children who were exposed to more child-directed speech were better able to identify familiar words during real-time language processing.
Mean proportion of trials on which children looked to the target picture, measured from the onset of the target noun. Black and grey lines represent children's looking time at 24 months, based on a median split of adult words directed to the child at 19 months. The top line shows the time course of looking behavior for children who heard more child-directed speech (CDS) at home; the lower line shows looking time for children who heard less CDS. The dashed vertical line represents target noun offset; error bars represent SEs over participants.
The three scatter plots (with best-fitting regression lines) show zero-order correlations between (a) vocabulary size (number of words) at 24 months and child-directed speech at home, (b) processing efficiency (mean percentage of time spent looking to the target picture) at 19 months and child-directed speech, and (c) vocabulary size at 24 months and processing efficiency at 19 months. The mediation model (d) shows the link between child-directed speech at 19 months and vocabulary size at 24 months, as mediated by processing efficiency at 19 months. Along the lower path, the solid and dashed arrows show results when the mediator was not included and was included in the model, respectively. Asterisks indicate significant paths (* p < .05, ** p < .01).
Can Differences in Processing Explain the Link between Language Experience and Vocabulary?
Next we ask whether the effect of language experience on processing efficiency helps explain the well-established relation between child-directed speech and vocabulary. We used mediation analysis to examine whether processing skill at 19 months accounted for the link between child-directed speech and 24-month vocabulary (while controlling for maternal education, recording length, and infant vocalizations at 19 months). The scatter plots in Fig. 3 illustrate the first three steps of the mediation analysis: 1) Exposure to child-directed speech at 19 months predicted vocabulary at 24 months (leftmost panel), 2) Exposure to child-directed speech also predicted processing efficiency at 19 months (middle panel), and 3) 19-month processing efficiency predicted 24-month vocabulary (rightmost panel), even when controlling for child-directed speech. Finally, a critical condition for mediation is that the path coefficient between the predictor variable (child-directed speech) and the outcome variable (vocabulary) be significantly reduced when the mediator variable (processing efficiency) is included in the model. As shown at the bottom of Figure 3 , the parameter estimate for the effect of child-directed speech on vocabulary was reduced from 12.61 to 7.41 when processing efficiency was included in the model. A bootstrap ( Preacher & Hayes, 2004 ) testing the significance of the indirect effect gave a 95% confidence interval (corrected for bias) of 0.44 to 13.61. This confirms that the mediation was significant, and suggests that language experience promotes vocabulary development at least in part via its influence on processing efficiency. The final model explained 47% of the variance in children's vocabularies at 24 months.
Are Differences in Processing Efficiency Explained by Infants’ Knowledge of the Target Words?
One potential concern is that some children may have been unfamiliar with some of the target words used in the study, in which case variability in processing efficiency might simply reflect differences in children's knowledge of these words. To control for this possibility, we collected an independent measure of each participant's familiarity with the target words. Using a list of only the words used in the study, parents were asked whether their child “understood” each target word. According to parents’ reports, all of the target words were understood by 66% of the children at 19 months, and 72% of the children at 24 months. For each child, we removed those trials with target words they were reported not to understand and then recomputed the processing efficiency measures. After re-running the mediation model reported above, the pattern of results remained the same: Child-directed speech was related to processing efficiency at 19 months ( r = 0.40, p < 0.05), and 19-month processing efficiency predicted vocabulary at 24 months ( r = 0.53, p < 0.01), even when controlling for CDS ( r = 0.41, p < 0.05). Finally, the parameter estimate for the effect of CDS on vocabulary was significantly reduced from 12.61 to 8.75 when processing efficiency was included in the model, indicating that processing efficiency mediated the link between child-directed speech and vocabulary.
In a final analysis we included only those children whose mean accuracy was greater than .50 at 19 months ( n = 22), thus excluding those whose performance was at or below the chance level overall. This analysis revealed even stronger correlations between child-directed speech and processing efficiency ( r = 0.58, p < 0.01), and between processing efficiency and later vocabulary ( r = 0.62, p < 0.01). Moreover, even in this smaller sample, processing efficiency mediated the link between CDS and vocabulary, i.e., the parameter estimate for the effect of child-directed speech on vocabulary was significantly reduced from 15.61 to 8.86 when processing efficiency was included in the model. These results provide further evidence that differences in processing efficiency do not simply reflect variability in children's all-or-none knowledge of the target words. Instead, differences in how quickly and reliably children interpret familiar words in real time reflect variability in a cognitive skill that facilitates further language learning.
This research yielded three main results: First, we found that variation in infants’ experience with child-directed speech in low-SES Spanish-speaking families predicted children's later vocabulary. This result replicates other studies linking caregiver speech and vocabulary development in low-SES children ( Hurtado et al., 2008 ; Pan et al., 2005 ), but goes beyond earlier research by using all-day recordings of daily interactions in the home to sample children's early language environments. Thus our measures of child-directed speech minimize potential artifacts introduced by the presence of an observer or by parents’ reactions to a laboratory setting. Second, by recording interactions with multiple family members and identifying different sources of adult speech accessible to the child, we found that it was only speech addressed directly to the infant, and not speech in adult conversations overheard by the child, that facilitated vocabulary learning at this age, consistent with recent studies of children in middle-class English-speaking families in the U.S. ( Shneidman, Arroyo, Levine, & Goldin-Meadow, 2012 ) and in Yucatec Mayan families ( Shneidman & Goldin-Meadow, 2012 ).
Third, the most important discovery in this research was that speech-processing efficiency mediated the relation between child-directed speech and vocabulary. This shows that a critical step in the path from early language experience to later vocabulary knowledge is the influence of language exposure on infants’ speech-processing skill. In previous studies, one explanation proposed for the association between exposure to more child-directed speech and faster vocabulary growth has been that more diverse language from caregivers provides children with more models to learn from as they begin to build a lexicon. Our findings reveal an additional mechanism by which differences in early language experience lead to differences in vocabulary size: Infants who hear more talk have more opportunities to interpret language, and to exercise skills such as segmenting speech and accessing lexical representations that are vital to word learning ( Saffran, Newport, & Aslin, 1996 ; Gershkoff-Stowe, 2002 ). As a result, infants with more exposure to child-directed speech are faster and more accurate to orient to familiar words in real-time, enabling them to learn new words more quickly and facilitating rapid vocabulary growth.
Our results also give rise to a challenging question: What factors explain the striking disparities observed between families in amount of verbal stimulation provided to infants? Studies comparing advantaged and disadvantaged families show that SES-differences are linked to variability both in speech and gesture directed to children and in children's language outcomes ( Hoff, 2003a ; Huttenlocher et al., 2010 ; Rowe & Goldin-Meadow, 2009 ). However, in such between-group comparisons, differences in caregiver input are confounded with many other factors associated with SES that could also lead to variability in language learning – such as parental education, access to resources, living in crowded conditions, and family stress levels ( Evans, 2004 ). By focusing here on differences within a homogeneous group of disadvantaged families, rather than on differences between SES groups, variability in these confounding factors was reduced. Given this more narrow focus, it was surprising to discover differences in amount of child-directed speech between families that were almost as large as those reported in the landmark study by Hart and Risley (1995) , whose sample spanned a broad demographic range from poverty-level to professional families. While they found significant differences between SES groups - with a 20-fold difference in verbal stimulation between those parents who were the most and least verbally engaged with their infants - our findings revealed an 18-fold difference in caregiver talk to infants within a demographically more homogeneous group of disadvantaged families. Moreover, the differences in parental engagement observed within this low-SES sample were not correlated with maternal education. An important implication of these findings is that although variability in parenting behaviors is consistently linked to factors related to SES, there is also considerable variability in parental verbal engagement that is independent of social class.
In ongoing research, we are exploring other factors that could explain observed differences in children's language environments. Previous studies have discussed several such factors, including variability in parents’ own verbal abilities or conversational style ( Hoff-Ginsberg, 1991 ), in the activities that parents tend to engage in with their children ( Hoff, 2003b ), and in parental stress and emotional well-being ( Conger, McCartey, Yang, Lahey, & Kropp, 1984 ). In addition, some studies have found that parents from different socio-cultural groups have different beliefs about the role they play in children's communicative development ( Heath, 1983 ), and Rowe (2008) found that parents’ knowledge and beliefs about child development mediated the relation between SES and caregiver speech to children. Although not assessed in the current study, parental beliefs are an important factor to consider in explaining differences in caregivers’ tendency to engage infants in language-rich interactions, given that these beliefs may be more malleable than other influential factors.
Our results reveal that caregiver talk has direct as well as indirect influences on lexical development. More exposure to child-directed speech not only provides more models for learning words but also sharpens infants’ emerging lexical processing skills, with cascading benefits for vocabulary learning. If increased opportunities for verbal interaction can strengthen critical processing skills that enable more efficient learning, then interventions aimed at increasing parents’ verbal engagement with their infants have the potential to change the course of vocabulary growth and, in turn, to improve later outcomes for disadvantaged children.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments.
This research was supported by a grant from the National Institutes of Health (R01 DC008838) to A. Fernald. We are grateful to the children and parents who participated. Special thanks to V. A. Marchman, R. Hoffmann Bion, R. D. Fernald, C. M. Fausey, and three anonymous reviewers for comments on earlier versions of the manuscript; and to N. Hurtado, L. Rodriguez Mata, C. Coon, M. Barraza, J. Villanueva, A. Arroyo, N. Otero, V. Limón, and L. Martinez and the staff of the Center for Infant Studies at Stanford University for help with data collection and coding.
AW and AF developed study concept and designed study. AW performed research and data analysis. AW and AF wrote the paper.
- Conger RD, McCarty JA, Yang RK, Lahey BB, Kropp JP. Perception of child, child-rearing values, and emotional distress as mediating links between environmental stressors and observed maternal behavior. Child Development. 1984; 55 :2234–2247. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Dale PS, Fenson L. Lexical development norms for young children. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers. 1996; 28 :125–127. [ Google Scholar ]
- Duncan GJ, Murnane RJ. Whither opportunity? Rising inequality, schools, and children's life chances. Russell Sage Foundation; New York, New York: 2011. [ Google Scholar ]
- Evans GW. The environment of childhood poverty. American Psychologist. 2004; 59 :77–92. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Fenson L, Dale PS, Reznick JS, Bates E, Thal DJ, Pethick SJ. Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1994; 59 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Fernald A, Marchman VA. Individual differences in lexical processing at 18 months predict vocabulary growth in typically developing and late-talking toddlers. Child Development. 2011; 83 :203–222. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Fernald A, Marchman VA, Weisleder A. SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science. 2013; 16 :234–248. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Fernald A, Perfors A, Marchman VA. Picking up speed in understanding: Speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. Developmental Psychology. 2006; 42 :98–116. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Fernald A, Zangl R, Portillo AL, Marchman VA. Looking while listening: Using eye movements to monitor spoken language comprehension by infants and young children. In: Sekerina IA, Fernandez EM, Clahsen H, editors. Developmental Psycholinguistics: On-line methods in children's language processing. J. Benjamins Pub.; Amsterdam; Philadelphia: 2008. [ Google Scholar ]
- Ford M, Baer CT, Xu D, Yapanel U, Gray S. The LENA Language Environment Analysis System: Audio Specifications of the DLP-0121. 2009:1–8. http://www.lenafoundation.org/TechReport.aspx/Audio_Specifications/LTR-03-2 .
- Gershkoff-Stowe L. Object naming, vocabulary growth, and the development of word retrieval abilities. Journal of Memory and Language. 2002; 46 (4):665–687. [ Google Scholar ]
- Hart BM, Risley TR. Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Brookes Publishing Co.; Baltimore, MD: 1995. [ Google Scholar ]
- Heath SB. Ways with words: Language, life, and work in communities and classrooms. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 1983. [ Google Scholar ]
- Hoff-Ginsberg E. Mother-child conversation in different social classes and conununicative settings. Child Development. 1991; 62 :782–796. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Hoff E. he specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development. 2003a; 74 :1368–1378. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Hoff E. Causes and consequences of SES-related differences in parent-to-child speech. In: Bornstein MH, Bradley RH, editors. Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development. Lawrence Erlbaum; Mahwah, NJ: 2003b. [ Google Scholar ]
- Hurtado N, Marchman VA, Fernald A. Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Developmental Science. 2008; 11 :F31–39. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Huttenlocher J, Waterfall H, Vasilyeva M, Vevea J, Hedges LV. Sources of variability in children's language growth. Cognitive Psychology. 2010; 61 :343–65. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Jackson-Maldonado D, Thal DJ, Marchman VA, Newton T, Fenson L, Conboy BT. MacArthur Inventarios del Desarrollo de Habilidades Comunicativas: User's guide and technical manual. Brookes Publishing Co.; Baltimore, MD: 2003. [ Google Scholar ]
- Lee VE, Burkam DT. Inequality at the starting gate: Social background differences in achievement as children begin school. Economic Policy Institute; Washington, DC: 2002. [ Google Scholar ]
- Marchman VA, Fernald A. Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Developmental Science. 2008; 11 :F9–16. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Newport EL, Gleitman LR, Gleitman H. Mother, I'd rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In: Snow CE, Fergus, editors. Talking to children: Language input and acquisition. Cambridge University Press; 1977. [ Google Scholar ]
- Oetting JB, Hartfield L, Pruitt S. Exploring LENA as a tool for researchers and clinicians. The ASHA Leader. 2009; 14 :20–22. [ Google Scholar ]
- Oliver BR, Plomin R. Twins’ Early Development Study (TEDS): A multivariate, longitudinal genetic investigation of language, cognition and behavior problems from childhood through adolescence. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2007; 10 :96–105. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Oller DK, Niyogi P, Gray S, Richards JA, Gilkerson J, Xu D, Yapanel U, et al. Automated vocal analysis of naturalistic recordings from children with autism, language delay, and typical development. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2010; 107 :13354–13359. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Pan BA, Rowe ML, Singer JD, Snow CE. Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development. 2005; 76 :763–82. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers. 2004; 36 :717–31. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Ramey CT, Ramey SL. Early learning and school readiness: Can early intervention make a difference? Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 2004; 50 :471–491. [ Google Scholar ]
- Reardon SF, Galindo C. The Hispanic-White achievement gap in Math and Reading in the elementary grades. American Educational Research Journal. 2009; 46 :853–891. [ Google Scholar ]
- Rowe ML. Child-directed speech: Relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of child language. 2008; 35 :185–205. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Rowe ML, Goldin-Meadow S. Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in child vocabulary size at school entry. Science. 2009; 323 :951–953. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Saffran JR, Newport EL, Aslin RN. Word segmentation: The role of distributional cues. Journal of Memory and Language. 1996; 35 :606–621. [ Google Scholar ]
- Shneidman LA, Arroyo ME, Levine S, Goldin-Meadow S. What counts as effective input for word learning? Journal of Child Language. 2012 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Shneidman LA, Goldin-Meadow S. Language input and acquisition in a Mayan village: How important is directed speech? Developmental Science. 2012; 15 :659–673. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Singh L, Steven Reznick J, Xuehua L. Infant word segmentation and childhood vocabulary development: a longitudinal analysis. Developmental Science. 2012; 15 :482–495. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Tsao F-M, Liu H-M, Kuhl PK. Speech perception in infancy predicts language development in the second year of life: a longitudinal study. Child Development. 2004; 75 :1067–84. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Xu D, Yapanel U, Gray S. Reliability of the LENA speech language environment analysis system in young children's natural home environment. 2009 http://www.lenafoundation.org/TechReport.aspx/Reliability/LTR-05-2 .
LINGUISTIC COMPETENCE OF GRADE SIX STUDENTS:BASIS FOR LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT PROGRAM FOR INTERMEDIATE LEARNERS
- EPRA International Journal of Environmental Economics Commerce and Educational Management
- This person is not on ResearchGate, or hasn't claimed this research yet.
Discover the world's research
- 25+ million members
- 160+ million publication pages
- 2.3+ billion citations
No full-text available
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the author.
- Recruit researchers
- Join for free
- Login Email Tip: Most researchers use their institutional email address as their ResearchGate login Password Forgot password? Keep me logged in Log in or Continue with Google Welcome back! Please log in. Email · Hint Tip: Most researchers use their institutional email address as their ResearchGate login Password Forgot password? Keep me logged in Log in or Continue with Google No account? Sign up
IMAGES
VIDEO
COMMENTS
Research shows that the amount of child-directed speech is a strong contributor to the child's language development (11, 27, 28). Based on clinical consensus in relation to these data, primary care clinicians play a role in primary prevention of language and speech disorders by counseling families about the importance of the learning environment.
New research on language acquisition, bilingualism, and speech perception.
A strong foundation in language skills is associated with positive, long-term academic, occupational, and social outcomes. Individual differences in the rate of language development appear early. Approximately 16% of children experience delays in initial phases of language learning; approximately half of those show persistent difficulties that may lead to clinical disorders.1 Because of the ...
To prevent misdiagnosed identification and boost assessment of language difficulties, more typical and atypical language studies are needed. In this sense, a good description of language acquisition could help to detect and prevent language difficulties. Nevertheless, most of the research on child language development has been conducted in English.
Explore the Language Development Research at Carnegie Mellon University, focusing on linguistic and cognitive development.
Language development is the process by which children come to understand and produce language to communicate with others, and entails the different components of phonology (sounds of a language), semantics (word meanings), grammar (rules on combining words into sentences), and pragmatics (the norms around communication).
Language is a complex human capacity. The speed with which young children learn it is a remarkable feat. Across diverse cultures and family structures, children learn thousands of languages. Despite tremendous variation across languages, commonalities hold in structure and learning mechanisms. Children undergo perceptual and expressive development. They learn to organize and process language ...
High-quality teacher-child interactions in early learning environments have been regarded as a key contributor to children's early language and cognitive development in international scholarships. Little is known, however, about the longitudinal ...
The effects of poor language on educational attainment and well-being raises the question of whether early interventions can improve language skills. Here, we review evidence showing that interventions delivered in school settings can improve children's oral language skills.
A defining property of human language is the creative use of words to express multiple meanings through word meaning extension. Such lexical creativity is manifested at different timescales, ranging from language development in children to the evolution of word meanings over history.
Young children learn to communicate in the language(s) of their communities, yet the individual trajectories of language development and the particular language varieties and modes of communication children acquire vary depending on the contexts in which they live. This review describes how context shapes language development. Building on the bioecological model of development, we ...
A tiered intervention model, graduating the level of provision in line with levels of need, has been posited as most appropriate for supporting children's language development. The first tier, or universal provision, is characterised by high-quality, evidence-informed language teaching for all.
Because such young language learners (YLLs) are in the process of drastic changes in their cognitive and language development, it is critically important to understand how YLLs develop cognition in relation to language so that we can develop cognitively and linguistically appropriate pedagogies, curriculum, and instructional materials for them.
This article critically interrogates the model of language that underpins early years policy and pedagogy. Our arguments emerge from an ethnographic study involving 2-year-olds attending a day care...
Abstract. Language development in children is a multifaceted process influenced by various factors, including biological predispositions, environmental influences, and cognitive processes. This ...
Early childhood teachers play a key role as children develop literacy. While this cluster does not cover the basics of reading instruction, it offers classroom-tested ways to make common practices like read alouds and discussions even more effective.
This book provides a comprehensive introduction to the topic of language development, taking a perspective that covers the period before birth through old age. There are between 4,000 and 6,000 ...
ABSTRACT The quality of a child's early language and communication environment (ELCE) is an important predictor of later educational outcomes. However, less is known about the routes via which these early experiences influence the skills that support academic achievement. Using data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (n = 7,120) we investigated relations between ELCE ...
Tracing the Roots of Language and Literacy. Researchers have long known that a child's brain develops the capacity for language rapidly and as a response to environmental input like listening to a story, talking with a caregiver, or looking at pages in a book. Up until now, though, the relationship between brain structure — specifically the ...
Language Development ! 2. The ability to communicate using language is one of the most basic human traits. It. involves learning to understand and produce an abstract and complex linguistic code ...
These studies show that children's language outcomes are linked both to early experience with language and to speech processing skills in infancy, but it is not well understood how these two influences work together over development to promote vocabulary growth.
The past 20 years have seen research on language acquisition in the cognitive sciences grow immensely. The current paper offers a fairly extensive review of this literature, arguing that new cognitive theories and empirical data are perfectly consistent with core predictions a behavior analytic approach makes about language development. The review focuses on important examples of productive ...
This paper attempts to do so in a selective manner by proposing a preliminary conceptual framework for second language emergent literacy development, thus paving the way for future research. The literature on the emergent literacy of native English speakers is first outlined, followed by a discussion of the general effects of bilingualism and ...
The onset of literacy marks a significant change in children's development. Written language is more complex than everyday conversation, and even books targeted at preschoolers contain more varied words and more complex syntax than child-directed speech does. We review the nature and content of children's book language, focusing on recent large-scale corpus analyses that systematically ...
We propose that it is important to make sure we use language that differentiates between delayed echolalia and the broader set of ideas that comprise gestalt language development. We also suggest that it is important to discuss the relationship between neurodiversity-affirmative practices and practices supported by research evidence.
Eventually, from the findings drawn out of the study a Language Development Program will be designed and proposed. This study employed the non-experimental descriptive survey research design in ...